08.09.2014 Views

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

students, whereas participants that did DATS in the vehicle cap simulator<br />

were recruited at local Department <strong>of</strong> Motor Vehicle <strong>of</strong>fices when people<br />

apply for their learner's permits.<br />

Finally, Wang, Zhang & Salvendy (2010) developed a simulator-based<br />

training program for <strong>novice</strong> <strong>drivers</strong> in which eight critical situations where<br />

embedded in the scenario; seven overt latent situations and only one covert<br />

latent hazard situation. After the training drive, each participant watched the<br />

video from the driver's point-<strong>of</strong>-view <strong>of</strong> his (only <strong>young</strong> male <strong>novice</strong> <strong>drivers</strong><br />

participated in this study) own performance in the critical situation and then<br />

viewed a video in which an experienced driver averted the hazards. To<br />

assess training retention, an evaluation drive on the same simulator was<br />

arranged six weeks after the training. The scenario <strong>of</strong> the evaluation drive<br />

differed from the scenario in the training drive. Four <strong>of</strong> the situations in the<br />

evaluation drive were near transfer situations and four were far transfer<br />

situation. The dependent variables were the scores on a 5-point scale by two<br />

independent assessors that were blind with regard to the condition. A score<br />

<strong>of</strong> 1 meant involvement in a crash and a score <strong>of</strong> 5 meant good hazard<br />

<strong>anticipation</strong>. Scores were significantly better for the trained group than for<br />

untrained group in six out <strong>of</strong> the eight critical situations. This was the case<br />

for both near transfer situations and for far transfer situations, but the effect<br />

was greater in the near transfer situations than in the far transfer situations.<br />

The results <strong>of</strong> the effect <strong>of</strong> simulator-based hazard <strong>anticipation</strong> training are<br />

not conclusive. The didactical methods ranged from mere one time exposure<br />

to immanent hazards (Regan et al., 1999; TRAINER., 2002) to a debriefing in<br />

which participants saw a video <strong>of</strong> their own performance and the<br />

performance <strong>of</strong> an expert (Wang et al., 2010). It seems that mere exposure to<br />

immanent hazards during a simulator drive is not sufficient. Participants<br />

have to be challenged to discover (by trial and error) why the critical event<br />

occurred and what they themselves can do to prevent it from happening the<br />

next time. It could be that Regan et al. (1999) did not find an effect whereas<br />

Ivancic & Hesketh (2000) did, because Ivancic & Hesketh (2000) made use <strong>of</strong><br />

the principles <strong>of</strong> error learning. Errors are usually salient, unexpected events<br />

that can motivate further learning about a task. The negative feedback<br />

provided by errors that are transparent to the individual who commits them,<br />

creates an element <strong>of</strong> surprise that temporarily halts task performance while<br />

learners try to work out why the error occurred. This is thought to delay the<br />

automatization <strong>of</strong> a skill and to increase the duration under which the task is<br />

performed using controlled processing (Kulhavy, 1977). Ideally, a simulator-<br />

205

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!