08.09.2014 Views

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

latent hazard. There were three versions <strong>of</strong> each scenario. First, they drove<br />

the scenario in which as in the video clips the latent hazard did not<br />

materialize. After this short drive, trainees were asked what could have<br />

happened that did not happen. Hereafter, irrespective <strong>of</strong> their answer they<br />

drove the so-called error drive. This error drive was the same drive as the<br />

first drive, but now with the latent hazard materializing aggressively. If the<br />

latent hazard was not detected and recognized, this drive ended in a crash or<br />

a near miss. After this, a plan view <strong>of</strong> the traffic situation appeared on the<br />

centre screen <strong>of</strong> the simulator. Trainees had to explain to themselves on the<br />

basis <strong>of</strong> this plan view why the near miss or crash had happened and what<br />

they could have done to avert the crash or near miss. Trainees also received<br />

instruction about how to scan and anticipate the latent hazard. Finally,<br />

trainees drove the scenario for the third time. In this third version, the latent<br />

hazard also materialized, but less aggressively than in the error drive. This<br />

third drive was intended to <strong>of</strong>fer trainees the opportunity to practice what<br />

they had learned. After this third drive, the cycle started all over again with a<br />

different latent hazard in a different scenario.<br />

In order to test if the training had improved visual search for latent<br />

hazards, eighteen trained and eighteen untrained <strong>young</strong> <strong>novice</strong> <strong>drivers</strong> that<br />

were around 19 years <strong>of</strong> age and had around two years driving experience,<br />

were evaluated on an advanced driving simulator. Participants drove<br />

through three scenarios that all together contained seven situations with<br />

latent hazards that did not materialize that were the same as the latent<br />

hazards in the training, but that were different in appearance. These were the<br />

near transfer situations. The three drives contained twelve situations with<br />

latent hazards that did not materialize that were conceptually different from<br />

the latent hazards in the training. These were the far transfer situations. The<br />

eye movements <strong>of</strong> participants were recorded while they drove. The trained<br />

group showed anticipatory gaze directions in 84% <strong>of</strong> the near transfer latent<br />

hazard situations and the untrained group showed correct gaze directions in<br />

57% <strong>of</strong> these situations. The trained group showed anticipatory gaze<br />

directions in 71% <strong>of</strong> the far transfer latent hazard situations and the<br />

untrained group showed anticipatory gaze directions in 53% <strong>of</strong> these<br />

situations. The differences between the groups in both the near transfer<br />

situations and the far transfer situations were significant, but the effect size<br />

was smaller in the far transfer situations. However, as far as the effect <strong>of</strong> the<br />

simulator-based training program could be compared with a PC-based<br />

training program that used the same latent hazards for training, the<br />

simulator-based training was not better than the PC-based training. Selfrating<br />

<strong>of</strong> driver confidence was not higher after the training for the trained<br />

270

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!