24.02.2013 Views

Encyclopedia of Evolution.pdf - Online Reading Center

Encyclopedia of Evolution.pdf - Online Reading Center

Encyclopedia of Evolution.pdf - Online Reading Center

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

altruism<br />

which the animal is not closely related. Reciprocal altruism<br />

will not work unless there is a minimal level <strong>of</strong> intelligence.<br />

This is because reciprocal altruism is susceptible to cheaters.<br />

There must be some punishment for the individual that keeps<br />

all its resources while accepting the help <strong>of</strong> others. The other<br />

animals need to have enough intelligence to remember who<br />

the cheaters are. This could be one <strong>of</strong> the major contributing<br />

factors in the evolution <strong>of</strong> human language and human<br />

intelligence. <strong>Evolution</strong>ary biologist Robin Dunbar suggests<br />

that language evolved largely because it allowed humans to<br />

keep track <strong>of</strong> the intricacies <strong>of</strong> social structure, which would<br />

include the ostracism <strong>of</strong> cheaters.<br />

In animal species with strong social hierarchies, the subordinate<br />

males receive no benefit for being altruistic toward<br />

the dominant males. Altruism between social classes conveys<br />

no benefit in those species. The males may, however, benefit<br />

greatly from carrying out acts <strong>of</strong> reciprocal altruism that win<br />

allies from their social peers. Because <strong>of</strong> the need both for<br />

paying back the altruism and for punishing cheaters, reciprocal<br />

altruism works best in animal species that are intelligent,<br />

social, and long-lived.<br />

Indirect Reciprocity<br />

Neither kin selection nor reciprocal altruism can explain<br />

altruism toward individuals who are unlikely or unable to<br />

reciprocate. While such altruism is rare in nonhuman species,<br />

it is very common among humans. <strong>Evolution</strong>ary biologist<br />

Ge<strong>of</strong>frey Miller points out that, in modern human charities,<br />

the recipient is <strong>of</strong>ten indigent and unable to reciprocate, and<br />

the recipient seldom knows who the donor is anyway. The<br />

donors <strong>of</strong>ten are not interested in the efficiency <strong>of</strong> resource<br />

transfer to the recipient. It would be much more efficient if<br />

a rich person continued to earn money, then donated that<br />

money, rather than working the equivalent number <strong>of</strong> hours<br />

in a soup kitchen. What, then, could be an evolutionary<br />

explanation for this kind <strong>of</strong> altruism?<br />

The key to this kind <strong>of</strong> altruism may be whether or not<br />

another animal is observing it. The altruist can obtain greater<br />

social stature by being altruistic toward individuals that are<br />

unrelated or that cannot repay. Mathematicians Karl Sigmund<br />

and Martin Nowak have produced calculations that<br />

demonstrate this advantage. Human donations <strong>of</strong> time and/or<br />

money to charities, says Ge<strong>of</strong>frey Miller, more closely resemble<br />

a display <strong>of</strong> wealth than a calculated plot to get reciprocal<br />

benefits. Altruism, like conspicuous consumption, may<br />

constitute a message to the population at large. Conspicuous<br />

charity proclaims, “I am rich enough to give away some <strong>of</strong><br />

my resources. This tells you that I am not only rich but also<br />

that I am a good person.” Conspicuous consumption tells the<br />

observers only the first <strong>of</strong> those two things. The reputation<br />

<strong>of</strong> being a good person might yield enough social benefits to<br />

compensate for the cost <strong>of</strong> the altruism.<br />

In particular, the altruist may gain advantages in mate<br />

choice. sexual selection could favor a conspicuous display<br />

<strong>of</strong> altruism, whether through charity or through a heroic deed<br />

to benefit the community. It is usually the males that display<br />

and the females that choose. Although among humans sexual<br />

selection has been more mutual, it is still the males who show<br />

<strong>of</strong>f, and the females who choose, more <strong>of</strong>ten than the other<br />

way around. Conspicuous altruism is not merely showing<br />

<strong>of</strong>f; it is actually useful information to the individual (usually<br />

the woman) making the choice <strong>of</strong> a mate: Such a man<br />

must have good resources and must be a good person who<br />

will be good to her. Displays <strong>of</strong> altruism need to be excessive,<br />

or prolonged, or both, in order for the woman to know<br />

the man is not faking it. Ge<strong>of</strong>frey Miller uses the example <strong>of</strong><br />

Ebenezer Scrooge, the character in a novel by British writer<br />

Charles Dickens. Before his transformation, Scrooge not only<br />

did not participate in kin selection (he was not generous to<br />

his nephew) or in reciprocal altruism (he was not generous to<br />

Bob Cratchett), but also it is no surprise that he was single.<br />

Sexual selection can, and routinely does, produce adaptations<br />

that are costly to the individual, whether it is human altruism<br />

or the tail <strong>of</strong> a bird <strong>of</strong> paradise. Miller uses sexual selection<br />

as an explanation not only <strong>of</strong> the peculiarly human excesses<br />

<strong>of</strong> altruism but all aspects <strong>of</strong> human intelligence (see intelligence,<br />

evolution <strong>of</strong>).<br />

Because altruism can provide fitness benefits, natural<br />

selection has also favored the evolution <strong>of</strong> emotions that<br />

reinforce altruism. Altruism feels good, in a number <strong>of</strong> ways,<br />

including feelings <strong>of</strong> satisfaction for being altruistic, gratitude<br />

toward donors, and rage toward cheaters. Neurobiologists<br />

have measured brain activity in human subjects involved in<br />

simulated situations <strong>of</strong> cooperation. They found that altruistic<br />

cooperation activated the same brain regions (such as the<br />

anteroventral striatum, also known as the pleasure center)<br />

as cocaine, beautiful faces, good food, and other pleasures.<br />

They also found this response when the subjects participated<br />

in sweet revenge against cheaters. The idea that the enjoyment<br />

<strong>of</strong> altruism has a natural basis is not new. American<br />

president Thomas Jefferson wrote in a letter to John Law in<br />

1814, “These good acts give pleasure, but how it happens<br />

that they give us pleasure? Because nature hath implanted in<br />

our breasts a love <strong>of</strong> others, a sense <strong>of</strong> duty to them, a moral<br />

instinct, in short, which prompts us irresistibly to feel and to<br />

succor their distresses.”<br />

<strong>Evolution</strong> can therefore explain the tendency toward<br />

altruism in three ways: kin selection, reciprocal altruism, and<br />

indirect reciprocity. Since it is the proclivity, rather than the<br />

act itself, which evolution explains, humans can perform individual<br />

acts <strong>of</strong> self-sacrifice that yield no fitness benefit. But if<br />

such acts were common, the tendency to perform them would<br />

be selected against. A person can sacrifice herself or himself<br />

in a totally unselfish fashion—and there are numerous examples<br />

<strong>of</strong> such saints and heroes—because the behavior pattern<br />

evolved in the human species as a result <strong>of</strong> people sacrificing<br />

themselves in a selfish fashion.<br />

<strong>Evolution</strong>ary altruism has also influenced the evolution <strong>of</strong><br />

ethical systems (see evolutionary ethics). Humans not only<br />

behave altruistically but believe that this is the right way to<br />

act. <strong>Evolution</strong>ary ethicist Michael Shermer indicates that, during<br />

the course <strong>of</strong> human evolution, feelings <strong>of</strong> affiliation with<br />

others and affection for others have evolved as reinforcements<br />

<strong>of</strong> altruism, first through kin selection within extended families<br />

and then through reciprocal altruism within communities.<br />

These feelings, being the product <strong>of</strong> natural selection, have a

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!