05.08.2013 Views

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

1. BASIC GRAMMATICAL FEATURES 107<br />

Pronom<strong>in</strong>al a xes <strong>in</strong> <strong>Mayan</strong> <strong>language</strong>s are marked for case, person<br />

and number, but not for gender or noun class. The adjunct NPs agree<br />

with the pronom<strong>in</strong>al a xes <strong>in</strong> person and number, but not <strong>in</strong> case<br />

or regard<strong>in</strong>g noun classi cation. However, lexical NPs are often not<br />

present. The reference for third person pronom<strong>in</strong>al a xes is retrieved<br />

from the preced<strong>in</strong>g discourse. Without an NP to agree with, it seems<br />

di cult to argue for agreement at all. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Mithun (1986b),<br />

the pronom<strong>in</strong>al a xes <strong>in</strong> <strong>language</strong>s like the <strong>Mayan</strong> <strong>language</strong>s are not<br />

agreement markers but must be identi ed as subject and object while<br />

lexical NPs represent<strong>in</strong>g subject and object function as appositive adjuncts.<br />

Mithun's hypothesis goes back at least to Humboldt's analysis<br />

of Nahuatl illustrated <strong>in</strong> example (49).<br />

Der Satz soll, se<strong>in</strong>er Form nach, schon im Verbum abgeschlossen<br />

ersche<strong>in</strong>en und wird nur nachher, gleichsam durch Apposition,<br />

naher bestimmt. 8 von Humboldt (1836/1963),<br />

531<br />

(49) ni-c-qua <strong>in</strong> nacatl<br />

1sSBJ-3sDO-eat the meat<br />

`I eat (it) the meat.' von Humboldt (1836/1963), 531<br />

In more recent literature, a similar analysis has been proposed by Jel<strong>in</strong>ek<br />

(1984) for Warlpiri. Follow<strong>in</strong>g von Humboldt (even though her<br />

work conta<strong>in</strong>s no reference to his work), Jel<strong>in</strong>ek proposes the follow<strong>in</strong>g<br />

de nition of non-con gurational <strong>language</strong>s:<br />

(50) The <strong>verb</strong>al complex constitutes a complete nite sentence by<br />

itself, consist<strong>in</strong>g of a <strong>verb</strong> and its arguments, while optionally<br />

occurr<strong>in</strong>g nom<strong>in</strong>als are analysed as non-argumental.<br />

The presence of cooccurr<strong>in</strong>g sets of referential elements with dist<strong>in</strong>ct<br />

syntactic functions, i.e. pronom<strong>in</strong>al a xes 9 and nom<strong>in</strong>als, is a de n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

feature of non-con gurational <strong>language</strong>s. I contend that <strong>Mayan</strong><br />

<strong>language</strong>s can be subsumed under this <strong>language</strong> type s<strong>in</strong>ce they have<br />

referential pronom<strong>in</strong>al a xes on the <strong>verb</strong> as well as nom<strong>in</strong>als. <strong>Akatek</strong><br />

additionally conforms to two of the associated optional features Jel<strong>in</strong>ek<br />

lists for this <strong>language</strong>s type:<br />

1. Independent pronouns are used primarily for emphatic contrastive<br />

reference.<br />

8 The sentence form is meant to appear self-conta<strong>in</strong>ed already <strong>in</strong> the <strong>verb</strong> form<br />

and is only then, via apposition, speci ed <strong>in</strong> more detail.<br />

9 Jel<strong>in</strong>ek calls them clitic pronouns.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!