05.08.2013 Views

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

148 5. PERCEPTION VERB COMPLEMENTS IN AKATEK<br />

but generally unnecessary, s<strong>in</strong>ce the reference for the possessor argument<br />

is retrieved from the NP preced<strong>in</strong>g the PVC, i.e. the subject NP<br />

of the PV, ix Mik<strong>in</strong>, is<strong>in</strong>terpreted as possessor as long as not speci ed<br />

otherwise.<br />

Example (28) shows a di erent organization of the complex construction:<br />

(28) x-;-y-il<br />

[ix Mik<strong>in</strong>] [s-mam] [y-el-toj]<br />

PERF-B3-A3-hear [NCL Micaela] [A3-father] [A3-leave-DIR]<br />

`Micaela saw her father leave.'<br />

Here, smam `her father' (the reference for the possessor `her' is retrieved<br />

from the preced<strong>in</strong>g NP), appears as the direct object NP of the matrix<br />

PV, while the type2 PVC follows, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the order VSO+Type2.<br />

The ability of matrix PVs <strong>in</strong> <strong>Akatek</strong> to take an object adjunct as<br />

well as the PVC is shown <strong>in</strong> more detail below. The subject NP of<br />

the complement predicate yeltoj <strong>in</strong> example (28) is retrieved from the<br />

preced<strong>in</strong>g clause, i.e. as long as noth<strong>in</strong>g else is speci ed, it is smam<br />

`her father'.<br />

Example (29) illustrates the alternative VOS argument order with<br />

atype2 PVC, which is an option as long as the type2 complement is<br />

comparatively short.<br />

(29) x-;-y-ab' [s-too s-mam] [ix Mik<strong>in</strong>]<br />

PERF-B3-A3-hear [A3-go A3-father] [NCL Micaela]<br />

`Micaela heard her father leave.'<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>in</strong> (30), smam ix Mik<strong>in</strong> `Micaela's father' is <strong>in</strong>terpreted as<br />

subject NP of the matrix predicate, while Micaela is the one leav<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

(30) x-;-y-ab' [s-mam ix Mik<strong>in</strong>] [s-too ix]<br />

PERF-B3-A3-hear [A3-father NCL Micaela] [A3-go she]<br />

`Micaela's father heard her leav<strong>in</strong>g.'<br />

If type2 <strong>complements</strong> are fronted, the result is an ungrammatical<br />

sentence as <strong>in</strong> (31). However, <strong>in</strong> section 3.1.7, I demonstrate that<br />

front<strong>in</strong>g by means of cleft<strong>in</strong>g of the whole PVC is grammatical.<br />

(31) *[a-ma'-on <strong>in</strong>-an] x-;-y-il<br />

[A2-hit-SUF B1-CL1s] PERF-B3-A3-see<br />

`You hit me and Micaela watched.'<br />

ix Mik<strong>in</strong><br />

NCL Micaela<br />

The other option to ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> grammaticality when front<strong>in</strong>g a PVC is<br />

to use a type1 PVC as <strong>in</strong> (32):

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!