05.08.2013 Views

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

178 5. PERCEPTION VERB COMPLEMENTS IN AKATEK<br />

questioned, relativized, or clefted, as long as at least one<br />

of the arguments was third person. The agentive voice<br />

<strong>verb</strong> cross-referenced the agent, though the patient is still<br />

expressed or implied with no overt modi cation.<br />

Smith-Stark's considerations did not <strong>in</strong>clude all aspects of antipassives,<br />

such as nom<strong>in</strong>alization (Smith-Stark, 1978). In sum, the diachronic<br />

analysis of the su x -on expla<strong>in</strong>s the synchronic usage of this su x <strong>in</strong><br />

question formation, relativization, and cleft <strong>in</strong> <strong>Akatek</strong>. Note that these<br />

syntactic processes only result <strong>in</strong> antipassive structures, i.e. <strong>in</strong> detransitivized<br />

constructions, when the subject is questioned, relativized, or<br />

clefted. The role of the -on <strong>in</strong> type2 <strong>complements</strong> and <strong>in</strong> discourse,<br />

where the <strong>verb</strong> is not detransitivized, rema<strong>in</strong>s unexpla<strong>in</strong>ed.<br />

Synchronically, *-(V)n developed <strong>in</strong>to a su x -ni <strong>in</strong> many <strong>Mayan</strong><br />

<strong>language</strong>s, e.g. <strong>in</strong> Jakaltek. In Jakaltek, the su x correspond<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

-on-i is -n(i). A synchronic analysis of the su x -n(i) <strong>in</strong> Jakaltek<br />

(Craig, 1976a) shows a high degree of overlap with the function of<br />

-on(-i) <strong>in</strong> <strong>Akatek</strong>. However, Craig (1976a) carefully dist<strong>in</strong>guishes<br />

between two homophonous -n(i) su xes. One marks operations on<br />

subjects of transitives, the other marks transitive type2 <strong>complements</strong><br />

and <strong>verb</strong>s of coord<strong>in</strong>ated transitive clauses. In the latter case both<br />

subject and object pronom<strong>in</strong>al a xes occur on the su xed <strong>verb</strong>, while<br />

<strong>in</strong> the former only the object pronom<strong>in</strong>al a x rema<strong>in</strong>s. This rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

of the object pronom<strong>in</strong>al a x on the <strong>verb</strong> is appropriate for <strong>in</strong>transitive<br />

<strong>verb</strong>s, which take set B a xes as their only arguments. As shown<br />

<strong>in</strong> the examples below, Jakaltek marks all operations on subjects of<br />

transitives, the set A pronom<strong>in</strong>al a x is deleted and -n(i) is su xed<br />

to the <strong>verb</strong> (Craig, 1976a). This is the case <strong>in</strong> relativization (120-a),<br />

cleft<strong>in</strong>g (120-b) and question formation (120-c).<br />

(120) a. x-;-ul naj x-;-'il-ni ix<br />

PERF-B3-arrive he PERF-B3-see-SUF she<br />

`He who saw her arrived.'<br />

b. ja'-; naj x-;-'il-ni ix<br />

CFT-B3 he PERF-B3-see-SUF she<br />

c.<br />

`It is he who saw her.'<br />

mac x-;-'il-ni ix<br />

who PERF-B3-see-SUF she<br />

`Who saw her?' Craig (1976a), 143f.<br />

The function of the -n(i) su x is to resolve the ambiguity result<strong>in</strong>g<br />

from the deletion or movement of post<strong>verb</strong>al NPs of transitive <strong>verb</strong>s<br />

(Craig, 1977). NPs are not marked for case. If both arguments are

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!