05.08.2013 Views

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

58 3. PERCEPTION VERB COMPLEMENTS IN ENGLISH<br />

<strong>in</strong>vestigate not only the <strong>in</strong>ternal structure (section 3) and the <strong>in</strong>ternal<br />

semantics (section 4) of PVCs, but also the question as to which<br />

complementtypes can occur with which k<strong>in</strong>d of matrix predicate. The<br />

orig<strong>in</strong>al observation of the <strong>in</strong>terdependency of complement types and<br />

matrix predicates was made by Vendler with regard to perfect and imperfect<br />

nom<strong>in</strong>als, i.e. PNOMs and INOMs, respectively. These two<br />

types of nom<strong>in</strong>alizations can be dist<strong>in</strong>guished by their di er<strong>in</strong>g ability<br />

to be embedded by two groups of matrix predicates which he termed<br />

narrow and loose conta<strong>in</strong>ers (Vendler, 1967). Narrow conta<strong>in</strong>ers<br />

designate matrix predicates which accept PNOMs but not INOMs<br />

or that-clauses, while loose conta<strong>in</strong>ers can take both PNOMs and IN-<br />

OMs as well as that-clauses. Both predicate classes conta<strong>in</strong> adjectival,<br />

<strong>verb</strong>al, or nom<strong>in</strong>al predicates. PNOMs can additionally occur with<br />

temporal prepositions. Examples for each conta<strong>in</strong>er type are provided<br />

<strong>in</strong> Table 4. The complement types (<strong>in</strong>dicated by X) function as either<br />

subjects or objects of the matrix predicates, i.e. of the conta<strong>in</strong>er predicates.<br />

For adjectival, postnom<strong>in</strong>al <strong>verb</strong>al and nom<strong>in</strong>al predicates, the<br />

<strong>complements</strong> function as subjects. Only for prenom<strong>in</strong>al <strong>verb</strong>al predicates<br />

do they function as objects.<br />

Table 4. Two classes of matrix predicates: narrow and<br />

loose conta<strong>in</strong>ers (Vendler, 1967)<br />

Narrow conta<strong>in</strong>ers Loose conta<strong>in</strong>ers<br />

(PNOM) (PNOM, INOM, that-clauses)<br />

adjectival Xbegradual/ slow/ Xbe unlikely/ probable/<br />

sudden/ fast/ prolonged certa<strong>in</strong><br />

postnom<strong>in</strong>al Xoccur/ take place/ X surprise s.o./ cause s.th.<br />

<strong>verb</strong>al beg<strong>in</strong>/ last/ end<br />

prenom<strong>in</strong>al see/ watch/ hear/ mention/ deny/ remember X<br />

<strong>verb</strong>al feel/ observe/ cause X<br />

nom<strong>in</strong>al Xbe an event/ Xbe a fact/ result of<br />

process/ action<br />

temporal PREP before/ after/ until X<br />

(note: X <strong>in</strong>dicates a subject or object complement.)<br />

In addition to active and passive perception <strong>verb</strong>s and aspectual<br />

expressions, which are discussed <strong>in</strong> more detail below, adjectival predicates<br />

such asXbe slow (26), nom<strong>in</strong>al predicates such asXbe a deliberate<br />

action (27), and certa<strong>in</strong> temporal prepositions such asuntil<br />

X (28) function as narrow conta<strong>in</strong>ers. The examples show that narrow<br />

conta<strong>in</strong>ers are de ned by accept<strong>in</strong>g PNOMs but not INOMs as<br />

<strong>complements</strong>.<br />

(26) a. *John's s<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g the Marseillaise was slow.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!