05.08.2013 Views

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

Perception verb complements in Akatek, a Mayan language

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

74 3. PERCEPTION VERB COMPLEMENTS IN ENGLISH<br />

However, with active perception <strong>verb</strong>s like watch a passive to construction<br />

is impossible as demonstrated <strong>in</strong> (80), an observation which<br />

supports the analysis that subjects of event denot<strong>in</strong>g PVCs cannot<br />

be passivized with <strong>in</strong> nitival to.<br />

(80) *John was watched to build a boat. Mittwoch (1990), 119<br />

Declerck (1983) presents further evidence aga<strong>in</strong>st the analysis that<br />

the passive with to is the passive counterpart of an active NI-PVC.<br />

He demonstrates that passives with <strong>in</strong> nitival to share structural and<br />

semantic properties of proposition denot<strong>in</strong>g to-PVCs and of that-<br />

PVCs but not those of event denot<strong>in</strong>g NI-PVCs.<br />

The typological relevance of passivization of PVs with di erent<br />

types of <strong>complements</strong> becomes apparent by look<strong>in</strong>g at other <strong>language</strong>s.<br />

Bennis & Hoekstra (1989) note that <strong>in</strong> <strong>language</strong>s that have passive<br />

<strong>in</strong> ections on the ma<strong>in</strong> <strong>verb</strong> as is the case <strong>in</strong> Swedish, the PVC<br />

subject can be passivized as illustrated <strong>in</strong> example (81-b). For passive<br />

constructions that consist of an auxiliary <strong>verb</strong> and a passive participle<br />

as e.g. <strong>in</strong> Dutch, English, and Swedish, the PVC subject cannot be<br />

passivized as demonstrated for Swedish <strong>in</strong> example (81-c).<br />

(81) a. Jag hor Peter sjunga en sang.<br />

I hear Peter s<strong>in</strong>g a song<br />

b. Peter hordes sjunga en sang.<br />

Peter was.heard s<strong>in</strong>g a song<br />

c. *Peter blev hord sjunga en sang.<br />

Bennis & Hoekstra (1989), 37<br />

My analysis of passivization processes <strong>in</strong> perception constructions <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>Akatek</strong> <strong>in</strong> chapter 5 supports this analysis. In <strong>Akatek</strong>, there are three<br />

morphological passives that can be applied to PVs with <strong>complements</strong>.<br />

The highly restricted ability of complex perception constructions <strong>in</strong><br />

English to passivize seems to be either the result of the lack of a morphological<br />

passive (Bennis & Hoekstra, 1989), or must be counted<br />

as idiosyncratic to English and other European <strong>language</strong>s.<br />

3.8.2. Complement-to-subject rais<strong>in</strong>g. Besides the passivization of<br />

the PV with S-to-S rais<strong>in</strong>g out of the PVC, the whole PVC can also be<br />

raised to the PV subject position (PVC-to-S rais<strong>in</strong>g). Clearly, NI-PVCs<br />

and to-PVCs cannot be passivized, while passivization of a that-PVC<br />

is grammatical:<br />

(82) a. *[John cook d<strong>in</strong>ner] was seen last night.<br />

b. *[Mary to be quali ed for the job] was seen by Jim.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!