23.02.2014 Views

Shape

Shape

Shape

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

406 Notes to p. 389<br />

artist’s hand is lost in impersonal line drawings on a computer. These are the things Langer considers:<br />

(1) openness to novelty; (2) alertness to distinction; (3) sensitivity to different contexts; (4) implicit, if not<br />

explicit, awareness of multiple perspectives; and (5) orientation in the present.<br />

My twin formulas for rule application guarantee number 5—‘‘orientation in the present.’’ <strong>Shape</strong>s<br />

divide and fuse every time a rule is tried, so that all past distinctions are lost in the present. What I<br />

see and do is what I see and do now, independent of anything that may have happened before.<br />

Parts vanish without a trace—they’re gone and forgotten. Identities in the schema x fi x are nice<br />

examples of this—try them as often as you like and see. But why stop when Langer’s four preceding<br />

points describe shapes and rules, too? And maybe there’s more in my copies of Langer’s copies<br />

of a few lines from the concluding part of William James’s third essay in The Meaning of Truth. Nitpicking<br />

here isn’t idle. Langer first quotes James so—<br />

Owing to the fact that all experience is a process, no point of view can ever be the last one. Every one is insufficient<br />

and off its balance, and responsible to later points of view than itself.<br />

—and then she quotes another passage to start a creative (design) process with rules in schemas<br />

like x fi tðxÞ and x fi xA. There’s James, then Langer’s copy in her book that distorts this in a subtle<br />

way, and then my own copy of her copy that changes it<br />

The standard [for what’s right] perpetually grows up endogenously inside the web of experience.<br />

and also a third copy ends my second paragraph on page 58. What James says may seem more<br />

important than how it’s copied, although copying what he says shows how it works. James sets<br />

the original standard with his own words, but this isn’t rigid. Its structure varies as it’s copied.<br />

The standard evolves and shifts—refined and distilled or changed entirely. And the preceding sentence<br />

is a new copy of James in the same ongoing process. (It’s funny how copying is used, and<br />

not just to design. In the Turing test for artificial intelligence, calculating machines are supposed<br />

to imitate you and me. But my way of copying with shapes and rules, and a shifting standard may<br />

alter this. At least copying—again in my way—allows for novelty and the reasoning it implies. All<br />

of this turns on the question I asked in the early pages of this book—what would calculating be<br />

like if Turing were a painter?) A permanent structure is an unproductive stricture. That’s because<br />

the letters u and i are side by side—inverse translations on my computer keyboard. Taking orthography<br />

too seriously only brakes/breaks the free flow of experience. I can derive the palindrome<br />

ababaababa or spell it from memory to rotate the three squares in the shape<br />

but does anyone really believe this happens before I erase and copy the squares, and what about<br />

other parts of the shape and its successors with five squares, seven, etc. if everything is spelled out<br />

ahead of time? Perhaps I can compile and use a dictionary of shapes with variants that are spelled<br />

differently. But then are some parts of shapes missing? How do shapes look? What an awful mess<br />

without embedding and mindful (visual) calculating! It’s lucky spelling is so easy to ignore, at least<br />

for scofflaws like James. (On James’s distaste for ‘‘the authority of prior use’’ and ‘‘obsolete verbal<br />

ritual,’’ namely, spelling—‘‘he didn’t even like the fact that everyone was expected to spell the<br />

same way’’—see L. Menand, The Metaphysical Club [New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux, 2001],<br />

88–89.) In fact, James tries copying, and our separate versions of it match exactly where they<br />

should. Langer’s two quotations follow a few pages after this:

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!