23.02.2014 Views

Shape

Shape

Shape

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

46 Introduction: Tell Me All About It<br />

Then, I’ve also tried to make the presentation visual in the way all of the ideas fit<br />

together. I like to think of this book as if it were a shape itself. There’s a lot of redundancy<br />

in shapes—pieces recur all over the place—and I’ve tried to do this with what I<br />

show and what I say about it. This doesn’t mean you’ll notice anything twice, but only<br />

that you’ll find the same thing in many different guises. There’s plenty of overlap and<br />

interpenetration of this kind. I’ve found it’s a useful way to describe how things work.<br />

Once you see that shapes and calculating with them aren’t about what you think,<br />

you’ll see what’s going on.<br />

There’s another way in which this book is like a shape. The different parts of a<br />

shape are shapes themselves. And the shape and its parts can stand alone. In the same<br />

way, the three parts of this book can be read separately, and for the most part are selfcontained.<br />

Each was written with the others in mind, and tried on different audiences.<br />

My guess is that if the parts work separately, then they’ll also work together. Whenever<br />

shapes combine, they fuse.<br />

Background<br />

The following background notes mostly follow the order of presentation in the preceding<br />

text. When coordinates seem necessary, I have indicated earlier page numbers. I<br />

thought of this when I saw ten-bars. You can find them on page 42.<br />

My use of references in these notes and elsewhere in this book deserves brief notice.<br />

To start with, my references aren’t comprehensive. Nor are they useful for their<br />

technical content, although it’s largely current. And they aren’t select points in the<br />

locus of a new debate. That visual and verbal expression are different doesn’t surprise<br />

anyone. No, the surprises are found in the relationships between seeing and calculating,<br />

and how they tie visual and verbal expression together. By and large, my references<br />

are about what it means to calculate—about what calculating is and isn’t<br />

supposed to involve. Some of the things I cite are so well known that they may seem<br />

gratuitous. This is a convenient way to highlight ideas that have become too obvious<br />

to discuss. In particular, nearly everyone I talk to naively assumes that generative processes<br />

are framed in combinatorial terms, so that calculating and seeing are both<br />

diminished. There’s a given vocabulary of symbols and rules to combine them. And<br />

some apply this to thinking (cognition)—‘‘[humans] have combinatorial minds.’’ 1 I<br />

try to show how there’s more. I also use references without regard to their original purpose.<br />

This is easy—it’s dealing with shapes. And it’s just as easy to trust anecdotal evidence<br />

before facts and results. This also has spatial aspects. It’s no good predicting<br />

what people will see and do next unless it shows how they’re free to go on in another<br />

way. Anything that can happen can be useful. In the end, what you get is how things<br />

appeared to me at the time. This is all you can ask for calculating with shapes. And<br />

what this shows is what I really want you to see. I have one thing foremost in mind—<br />

to explain in whatever way I can what it’s like to calculate by seeing and why this is<br />

different than counting. Design and calculating with shapes are much the same. The<br />

trick is to be creative. This is a licentious process in which nothing is fixed. There’s

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!