19.05.2014 Views

Reading Working Papers in Linguistics 4 (2000) - The University of ...

Reading Working Papers in Linguistics 4 (2000) - The University of ...

Reading Working Papers in Linguistics 4 (2000) - The University of ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

K ALJENAIE<br />

mascul<strong>in</strong>e forms are considered the unmarked forms <strong>in</strong> Arabic, <strong>in</strong> present<br />

and past tense respectively, and these forms persisted dur<strong>in</strong>g the data<br />

collection period. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly enough, children reta<strong>in</strong> the CCV root<br />

structure. S<strong>in</strong>ce f<strong>in</strong>iteness <strong>in</strong> Arabic is marked by Tense and Agreement, it<br />

is possible to call the mascul<strong>in</strong>e imperative and the 3 rd mascul<strong>in</strong>e past tense<br />

verb forms non-f<strong>in</strong>ite forms, especially s<strong>in</strong>ce there is no overt tense<br />

mark<strong>in</strong>g. However, <strong>in</strong> this paper the term unmarked is used <strong>in</strong>stead.<br />

It is important to take <strong>in</strong>to account, however, that all the variation<br />

noticed <strong>in</strong> the four children language was with<strong>in</strong> well-def<strong>in</strong>ed limits. <strong>The</strong><br />

children did not construct “wild grammars”. Based on the assumption that<br />

children are equipped with <strong>in</strong>nate pr<strong>in</strong>ciples <strong>of</strong> UG, each “<strong>in</strong>termediate<br />

grammar” must be constra<strong>in</strong>ed with these pr<strong>in</strong>ciples (Hyams, 1986).<br />

Moreover, Grodz<strong>in</strong>sky (1990) suggests that children’s language cannot be<br />

unacceptable str<strong>in</strong>gs accord<strong>in</strong>g to the adult grammar. Hyams (1987)<br />

proposes a Stem Parameter which is a well-formedness condition on word<br />

formation: ‘a verbal stem does/does not constitute a well formed word’.<br />

Concern<strong>in</strong>g acquisition, children “set” this parameter very early on, so they<br />

learn at a young age what is a well-formed word <strong>in</strong> their language. S<strong>in</strong>ce<br />

Arabic is a highly synthetic language with rich bound morphology, neither<br />

root nor stems (sometimes) can be used as <strong>in</strong>dividual entities, it is<br />

reasonable to assume that prior to a more advanced stage <strong>of</strong> language<br />

process<strong>in</strong>g, children would not treat stems as <strong>in</strong>dependent items.<br />

<strong>The</strong>refore, <strong>in</strong> unmark<strong>in</strong>g verb forms, children have two options. <strong>The</strong>y<br />

either leave the form un<strong>in</strong>flected, or alternatively they choose another<br />

<strong>in</strong>fix. <strong>The</strong> former option is excluded s<strong>in</strong>ce stems do not constitute a wellformed<br />

word <strong>in</strong> their language. <strong>The</strong> other option is to mark the stem with<br />

another <strong>in</strong>flection. Both options satisfy the requirement obliged by the<br />

Stem Parameter accord<strong>in</strong>g to the language, however the second one<br />

violates an agreement rule with the subject.<br />

Hyams (1986) acknowledges that although children <strong>in</strong> their early<br />

stages <strong>of</strong> acquisition do not employ <strong>in</strong>flections <strong>in</strong> an orderly way, the nonappearance<br />

<strong>of</strong> class errors implies that they identify which grammatical<br />

<strong>in</strong>flections belong to which lexical class. It strongly suggests that children<br />

have no problem <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g morpheme boundaries where grammatical<br />

<strong>in</strong>flections are used with word stems from quite early which suggests that<br />

both the stem forms and their <strong>in</strong>flections are evidently psychologically real<br />

units (Slob<strong>in</strong>, 1966, 1973). <strong>The</strong> data shows that stem errors where children<br />

use the appropriate <strong>in</strong>flection with the wrong stem are strik<strong>in</strong>gly rare (cf.<br />

Table 8).<br />

22

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!