19.05.2014 Views

Reading Working Papers in Linguistics 4 (2000) - The University of ...

Reading Working Papers in Linguistics 4 (2000) - The University of ...

Reading Working Papers in Linguistics 4 (2000) - The University of ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

R. INGHAM<br />

good as their CA matches on this measure <strong>of</strong> a-structure performance, vary<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the structure to suit the scene be<strong>in</strong>g depicted. This could suggest that a-<br />

structure knowledge is acquired as part <strong>of</strong> grammatical development, rather<br />

than be<strong>in</strong>g dissociated from it.<br />

In fact, <strong>in</strong> the Schelletter et al. study, the children with a morphological<br />

deficit also had a much greater lexical deficit as measured by BPVS scores<br />

(av. 25 months versus av. 16 months) so their poor argument structure<br />

performance may simply have been a reflection <strong>of</strong> the lexical deficit rather<br />

than with the grammatical deficit. In the present study this issue was taken up<br />

by look<strong>in</strong>g at two SLI groups very sharply differentiated as regards lexical<br />

knowledge. If argument structure alternations are a matter <strong>of</strong> lexical learn<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

then we would expect the LD group to have a deficit <strong>in</strong> argument structure<br />

realisation as compared with vocabulary matches, but the NLD group to have<br />

no such deficit as compared with their matches.<br />

3. Methodology<br />

<strong>The</strong> performance <strong>of</strong> these four groups was <strong>in</strong>vestigated on elicitation tasks<br />

designed to reveal aspects <strong>of</strong> the grammatical and lexical abilities <strong>of</strong> preschool<br />

children. <strong>The</strong>se are described <strong>in</strong> detail <strong>in</strong> Fletcher et al. (submitted) &<br />

Ingham et al. (1999). <strong>The</strong>se <strong>in</strong>volved describ<strong>in</strong>g two series <strong>of</strong> video scenes.<br />

<strong>The</strong> first task used a commercially available video from the P<strong>in</strong>gu series,<br />

which has no words but plenty <strong>of</strong> actions. It was played twice. <strong>The</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>vestigator described events <strong>in</strong> the present tense the first time the video was<br />

shown. <strong>The</strong> second time, children re-narrated the story <strong>in</strong> the past tense. This<br />

task allowed certa<strong>in</strong> grammatical areas associated with f<strong>in</strong>iteness, such as<br />

tense form, to be <strong>in</strong>vestigated, and was chosen to allow plenty <strong>of</strong><br />

opportunities for the use <strong>of</strong> regular past verbs.<br />

<strong>The</strong> second task used a video especially prepared for the research<br />

project, and featured scenes designed to illustrate actions denoted by verbs <strong>of</strong><br />

particular <strong>in</strong>terest for their argument structure. Five verbs which were<br />

alternated by 50% or more <strong>of</strong> adult controls were scored for the use <strong>of</strong><br />

alternations by the 40 SLI and LN children. <strong>The</strong>se were open, move, bounce,<br />

give and rub. <strong>The</strong> child was supplied with the target verb, and asked to say<br />

what happened <strong>in</strong> the video scene. For each verb two scenes were <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong><br />

the video; <strong>in</strong> each scene the events portrayed were such as to favour the use <strong>of</strong><br />

one alternant rather than the other. For example, <strong>in</strong> one scene for bounce an<br />

52

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!