12.12.2012 Views

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

96 brains<br />

<strong>The</strong> medial prefrontal cortex may thus serve as an interface between<br />

cognitive and emotional systems, allowing cognitive information processing<br />

in <strong>the</strong> prefrontal cortex to regulate emotional processing by <strong>the</strong> amygdala.<br />

In addition, emotional processing by <strong>the</strong> amygdala may influence decision<br />

making and o<strong>the</strong>r cognitive functions of <strong>the</strong> prefrontal cortex. Consequently,<br />

prefrontal–amygdala interactions may be involved in <strong>the</strong> conscious feelings<br />

of fear (see section below).<br />

In humans, damage to <strong>the</strong> amygdala interferes with implicit emotional<br />

memories but not explicit memories about emotions, whereas damage to<br />

<strong>the</strong> medial temporal lobe memory system interferes with explicit memories<br />

about emotions but not with implicit emotional memories (Bechara,<br />

Damasio, & Damasio, 2003; LaBar, Crupain, Voyvodic, & McCarthy, 2003).<br />

While explicit memories with and without emotional content are formed<br />

by way of <strong>the</strong> medial temporal lobe, those with emotional content differ from<br />

those without such content. <strong>The</strong> former tend to be longer-lasting and more<br />

vivid (Christianson, 1992; Cahill & McGaugh, 1998). Lesions of <strong>the</strong> amygdala<br />

or systemic administration of a b-adrenergic antagonist prevent this amplifying<br />

effect of emotion on declarative memory (Cahill & McGaugh, 1998),<br />

suggesting that <strong>the</strong> amygdala can modulate <strong>the</strong> storage of explicit memories<br />

in cortical areas. At <strong>the</strong> same time, <strong>the</strong> medial temporal lobe memory system<br />

projects to <strong>the</strong> amygdala (see above). Retrieval of long-term memories<br />

of traumatic events may trigger fear reactions by way of <strong>the</strong>se projections to<br />

<strong>the</strong> amygdala.<br />

WHAT ABOUT FEELINGS?<br />

Consciousness<br />

Our discussion above of <strong>the</strong> relation between <strong>the</strong> study of emotion in rats<br />

and humans brings us at last to <strong>the</strong> issue of how <strong>the</strong> conscious dimension of<br />

“feelings” in humans relates to <strong>the</strong> processing approach that has proved so<br />

successful in <strong>the</strong> study of rats. Subjective emotional experience, like <strong>the</strong><br />

feeling of being afraid, results when we become consciously aware that an<br />

emotion system of <strong>the</strong> brain, like <strong>the</strong> defense system, is active. In order for<br />

this to occur, we need at least two things: a defense system and <strong>the</strong> capacity<br />

to be consciously aware of its activity. <strong>The</strong> up side of this line of thought is<br />

that once we understand consciousness, we will also understand subjective<br />

emotional experiences. Many believe that <strong>the</strong> down side is that in order to<br />

understand subjective emotional experiences, we need to understand consciousness.<br />

However, it might be argued that our “divide-and-conquer” ap-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!