12.12.2012 Views

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

346 conclusions<br />

approach to <strong>the</strong> social interactions stressed by Adolphs (Chapter 2). Indeed,<br />

Jeannerod (Chapter 6) explores <strong>the</strong> possible role of mirror systems in empathy<br />

and our ability to understand <strong>the</strong> emotions of o<strong>the</strong>rs. However, I must<br />

confess here that <strong>the</strong> current chapter will place most emphasis on <strong>the</strong> brainin-<strong>the</strong>-individual<br />

approach and will conclude by giving a <strong>the</strong>ory of robot<br />

emotions grounded in <strong>the</strong> analysis of a robot going about its tasks in some<br />

ecological niche, ra<strong>the</strong>r than emphasizing social interactions.<br />

Vision Evolving<br />

<strong>The</strong> year 1959 saw <strong>the</strong> publication of two great papers on <strong>the</strong> neurophysiology<br />

of vertebrate vision: <strong>the</strong> study by Lettvin, Maturana, McCulloch, and<br />

Pitts (1959) of feature detectors in <strong>the</strong> frog’s retina and that by Hubel and<br />

Wiesel (1959) of receptive fields of neurons in <strong>the</strong> cat primary visual cortex.<br />

We will analyze <strong>the</strong> first work in relation to later studies of frog behavior<br />

(postponing a brief look at <strong>the</strong> role of motivation; we will <strong>the</strong>n look at<br />

<strong>the</strong> more generic coding in <strong>the</strong> cat visual system and ponder its implications.<br />

Action-Oriented Feature Detectors in Frog Retina<br />

Lettvin, Maturana, McCulloch, and Pitts (1959) studied “what <strong>the</strong> frog’s eye<br />

tells <strong>the</strong> frog’s brain” and reported that frog ganglion cells (<strong>the</strong> output cells<br />

of <strong>the</strong> retina) come in four varieties, each providing a retinotopic map of a<br />

different feature to <strong>the</strong> tectum, <strong>the</strong> key visual region of <strong>the</strong> midbrain (<strong>the</strong><br />

homolog, or “evolutionary cousin,” of what in mammals is often referred to<br />

as <strong>the</strong> “superior colliculus”):<br />

1. <strong>The</strong> boundary detectors<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> movement-gated, dark convex boundary detectors<br />

3. <strong>The</strong> moving or changing contrast detectors<br />

4. <strong>The</strong> dimming detectors<br />

Indeed, axons of <strong>the</strong> cells of each group end in a separate layer of <strong>the</strong><br />

tectum but are in registration: points in different layers which are stacked<br />

atop each o<strong>the</strong>r in <strong>the</strong> tectum correspond to <strong>the</strong> same small region of <strong>the</strong><br />

retina. All this shows that <strong>the</strong> function of <strong>the</strong> frog retina is not to transmit<br />

information about <strong>the</strong> point-to-point pattern distribution of light upon it<br />

but ra<strong>the</strong>r to analyze this image at every point in terms of boundaries, moving<br />

curvatures, changing contrasts, and local dimming. Lettvin’s group argues<br />

that <strong>the</strong> convexity detectors (operation 2 above) serve as “bug perceivers,”<br />

while operation 4 could be thought of as providing “predator detectors.”

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!