12.12.2012 Views

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

an evolutionary <strong>the</strong>ory of emotion 123<br />

(unlearned) reinforcers do not produce emotions, whereas secondary reinforcers<br />

(stimuli associated by stimulus–reinforcement learning with primary<br />

reinforcers) do. <strong>The</strong>y describe <strong>the</strong> pain as a sensation, but neutral stimuli<br />

(e.g., a table) can produce sensations when touched. It accordingly seems to<br />

be much more useful to categorize stimuli according to whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>y are<br />

reinforcing (in which case <strong>the</strong>y produce emotions) or not (in which case <strong>the</strong>y<br />

do not produce emotions). Clearly, <strong>the</strong>re is a difference between primary<br />

reinforcers and learned reinforcers; and operationally, it is whe<strong>the</strong>r a stimulus<br />

is reinforcing that determines whe<strong>the</strong>r it is related to emotion. A third<br />

issue is that, as we are about to see, emotional states (i.e., those elicited by<br />

reinforcers) have many functions, and <strong>the</strong> implementations of only some of<br />

<strong>the</strong>se functions by <strong>the</strong> brain are associated with emotional feelings, that is,<br />

with conscious emotional states (Rolls, 1999a). Indeed, <strong>the</strong>re is evidence for<br />

interesting dissociations in some patients with brain damage between actions<br />

performed to reinforcing stimuli and what is subjectively reported. In this<br />

sense, it is biologically and psychologically useful to consider that emotional<br />

states include more than those states associated with conscious feelings of<br />

emotion (Rolls, 1999a).<br />

THE FUNCTIONS OF EMOTION<br />

<strong>The</strong> functions of emotion also provide insight into <strong>the</strong> nature of emotion.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se functions, described more fully elsewhere (Rolls, 1990, 1999a), can<br />

be summarized as follows:<br />

1. Elicitation of autonomic responses (e.g., a change in heart rate)<br />

and endocrine responses (e.g., <strong>the</strong> release of adrenaline). While<br />

this is an important function of emotion, it is <strong>the</strong> next function<br />

that is crucial in my evolutionary <strong>the</strong>ory of why emotion is so<br />

important.<br />

2. Flexibility of behavioral responses to reinforcing stimuli. Emotional<br />

(and motivational) states allow a simple interface between sensory<br />

inputs and action systems. <strong>The</strong> essence of this idea is that<br />

goals for behavior are specified by reward and punishment<br />

evaluation and that innate goals are specified by genes. When<br />

an environmental stimulus has been decoded as a primary reward<br />

or punishment or (after previous stimulus–reinforcer association<br />

learning) a secondary one it becomes a goal for action.<br />

<strong>The</strong> animal can <strong>the</strong>n perform any action (instrumental response)<br />

to obtain <strong>the</strong> reward or avoid <strong>the</strong> punishment. <strong>The</strong> instrumental<br />

action, or operant, is arbitrary and could consist of a left turn

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!