12.12.2012 Views

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

152 brains<br />

Movements performed by living organisms owe <strong>the</strong>ir specificity to <strong>the</strong><br />

fact that <strong>the</strong>y usually have a goal. As a consequence, <strong>the</strong>y display a number<br />

of kinematic properties that reveal <strong>the</strong>ir “intentional” origin. One of <strong>the</strong>se<br />

properties is that goal-directed movements have an asymmetrical kinematic<br />

profile—a fast acceleration followed by a much longer deceleration—as opposed<br />

to <strong>the</strong> symmetrical profile of <strong>the</strong> ballistic motion of a projectile, for<br />

example. Ano<strong>the</strong>r property is that <strong>the</strong> tangential velocity of <strong>the</strong> moving limb<br />

varies with <strong>the</strong> radius of curvature of <strong>the</strong> movement (Lacquaniti, Terzuolo, &<br />

Viviani, 1983). A fur<strong>the</strong>r characteristic of biological movements is that <strong>the</strong>y<br />

follow biomechanically compatible trajectories. Consider <strong>the</strong> perceptual effect<br />

produced by fast sequential presentation of pictures of an actor with an<br />

arm at two different postures. This alternated presentation is perceived as a<br />

continuous apparent movement between <strong>the</strong> two arm postures. If, however,<br />

<strong>the</strong> presentation of <strong>the</strong> two postures is such that <strong>the</strong> arm should go across<br />

an obstacle (e.g., ano<strong>the</strong>r body part), <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> apparent movement is perceived<br />

as going around and not across <strong>the</strong> obstacle. This striking effect<br />

(Shiffrar & Freyd, 1990) reflects <strong>the</strong> implicit representation built from visual<br />

perception of motion when it refers to a biological (or intentional) origin.<br />

Obviously, this is not to say that a robot could not be programmed for<br />

accurately reaching a goal with a different strategy (e.g., using movements<br />

with a symmetrical velocity profile or violating biomechanical constraints):<br />

<strong>the</strong>se movements would simply look “unnatural” and would not match <strong>the</strong><br />

internal representation that a human subject has of an intentional movement.<br />

As a matter of fact, a normal subject cannot depart from <strong>the</strong> relation<br />

between geometry and kinematics which characterizes biological action: he<br />

or she cannot track a target moving with a spatiotemporal pattern different<br />

from <strong>the</strong> biological one (e.g., accelerating ra<strong>the</strong>r than decelerating in <strong>the</strong><br />

curves). According to Viviani (1990), <strong>the</strong> subject’s movements during <strong>the</strong><br />

attempts to track <strong>the</strong> target “continue to bear <strong>the</strong> imprint of <strong>the</strong> general<br />

principle of organization for spontaneous movements, even though this is<br />

in contrast with <strong>the</strong> specifications of <strong>the</strong> target.” Interestingly, <strong>the</strong> same<br />

relation between velocity and curvature is also present in a subject’s perceptual<br />

estimation of <strong>the</strong> shape of <strong>the</strong> trajectory of a luminous target. A target<br />

moving at a uniform velocity is paradoxically seen as moving in a nonuniform<br />

way and, conversely, a kinematic structure which respects <strong>the</strong> above<br />

velocity–curvature relation is <strong>the</strong> condition for perceiving a movement at a<br />

uniform velocity. According to Viviani and Stucchi (1992), perception is<br />

constrained by <strong>the</strong> implicit knowledge that <strong>the</strong> central nervous system has<br />

concerning <strong>the</strong> movements that it is capable of producing. In o<strong>the</strong>r words,<br />

<strong>the</strong>re is a central representation of what a uniform movement should be,<br />

and this representation influences visual perception. Whe<strong>the</strong>r this implicit<br />

knowledge is a result of learning (e.g., by imitation) or an effect of some innate

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!