12.12.2012 Views

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

150 brains<br />

Specific methods, partly based on introspection but also relying on<br />

changes of physiological variables, have been designed to experimentally<br />

access <strong>the</strong>se mental states characterized by absence or paucity of overt behavior.<br />

One of <strong>the</strong> most extensively studied of <strong>the</strong>se representational aspects<br />

of action is mental motor imagery. Behavioral studies of motor imagery have<br />

revealed that motor images retain <strong>the</strong> same temporal characteristics as <strong>the</strong><br />

corresponding real action when it comes to execution. For example, it takes<br />

<strong>the</strong> same time to mentally “walk” to a prespecified target as it takes to actually<br />

walk to <strong>the</strong> same place (Decety, Jeannerod, & Prablanc, 1989). Similarly,<br />

temporal regularities which are observed in executed actions, such as<br />

<strong>the</strong> classical speed–accuracy tradeoff, are retained in <strong>the</strong>ir covert counterparts<br />

(Sirigu et al., 1996). Along <strong>the</strong> same line, o<strong>the</strong>r situations have been<br />

described where <strong>the</strong> subject uses a motor imagery strategy in spite of <strong>the</strong><br />

fact that no conscious image is formed. Those are situations where <strong>the</strong> subject<br />

is requested to make a perceptually based “motor” decision. Consider,<br />

for example, <strong>the</strong> situation where a subject is simply requested to make an<br />

estimate about <strong>the</strong> feasibility of an action, like determining <strong>the</strong> feasibility<br />

of grasping an object placed at different orientations: <strong>the</strong> time to give <strong>the</strong><br />

response will be a function of <strong>the</strong> object’s orientation, suggesting that <strong>the</strong><br />

arm has to be mentally moved to an appropriate position before <strong>the</strong> response<br />

can be given. Indeed, <strong>the</strong> time to make this estimate is closely similar<br />

to <strong>the</strong> time it takes to actually reach and grasp an object placed at <strong>the</strong> same<br />

orientation (Frak, Paulignan, & Jeannerod, 2001; see also Parsons, 1994).<br />

One may speculate whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> same isochrony would also exist for performing<br />

an action with a disembodied artifact (e.g., a car) and mentally<br />

estimating its consequences. <strong>The</strong> question would be whe<strong>the</strong>r one can simulate<br />

an action performed, not by a human body, but with a mechanical<br />

device. A tentative answer will be given below.<br />

This indication of a similar temporal structure for executed and nonexecuted<br />

actions by a biological system is reinforced by a similarity at <strong>the</strong><br />

level of physiological indicators. Examining autonomic activity in subjects<br />

imagining an action at different effort rates reveals changes in heart rate and<br />

respiration frequency proportional to <strong>the</strong> imagined effort in <strong>the</strong> absence of<br />

any metabolic need. <strong>The</strong>se results (Decety, Jeannerod, Durozard, & Baverel,<br />

1993, see review in Jeannerod, 1995) reveal <strong>the</strong> existence of a central patterning<br />

of vegetative commands during covert actions, which would parallel<br />

<strong>the</strong> preparation of muscular commands. Autonomic changes occurring<br />

during motor imagery are closely related to those observed during central<br />

preparation of an effortful action (Krogh & Lindhard, 1913). Those are<br />

mechanisms that anticipate forthcoming metabolic needs, with <strong>the</strong> function<br />

of shortening <strong>the</strong> intrinsic delay required for heart and respiration to adapt<br />

to effort (e.g., Adams, Guz, Innes, & Murphy, 1987).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!