12.12.2012 Views

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

how do we decipher o<strong>the</strong>rs’ minds? 155<br />

to <strong>the</strong> observed action and, thus, to having <strong>the</strong> possibility of being delayed<br />

with respect to <strong>the</strong> observed action.<br />

In <strong>the</strong> context of emotions, arguments for dissociating <strong>the</strong> recognition<br />

of an emotion (as in contagion) from <strong>the</strong> recognition of <strong>the</strong> cause of that<br />

emotion (and <strong>the</strong> appropriate response to be given to it) are twofold. First,<br />

very young babies (at around 5 months of age) are able to recognize that a<br />

person is expressing an emotion, although <strong>the</strong>y do not seem to experience<br />

this emotion <strong>the</strong>mselves. It is only later (at 2 years of age) that <strong>the</strong>y begin to<br />

respond to <strong>the</strong> emotions expressed by o<strong>the</strong>r people (e.g., Nelson, 1987). In<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r words, it is possible to recognize an emotion without having it. <strong>The</strong><br />

second argument is that pure contagion would not yield an appropriate response.<br />

Imagine facing somebody who expresses anger and threat. <strong>The</strong> appropriate<br />

response is not to experience anger oneself but to experience fear<br />

and perhaps to run away. Because contagion cannot tell what <strong>the</strong> emotion is<br />

about, it cannot be used as a useful means for reacting to o<strong>the</strong>rs’ emotions.<br />

Contagion of action and emotion would act as a relatively primitive form of<br />

recognition of o<strong>the</strong>rs’ behavior, with a role in activating imitation mechanisms,<br />

particularly in young infants, a behavior that becomes progressively<br />

inhibited at later stages. Pathology offers an example of disinhibition of compulsive<br />

imitation in patients with frontal lobe lesions (Lhermitte, 1983).<br />

Ano<strong>the</strong>r way of getting close to o<strong>the</strong>rs’ minds is empathy. <strong>The</strong> concept<br />

of empathy is more clearly related to <strong>the</strong> idea of simulation than contagion<br />

in that it implies that individuals involved in a given interaction share a similar<br />

mental state. In contradistinction to contagion, empathy requires that one<br />

has information on <strong>the</strong> experience and intentions of <strong>the</strong> person who is observed<br />

and whose mental content one is attempting to understand. Observing<br />

a person about whose intentions, thoughts, or experience one has no<br />

information would provide only a limited access to what <strong>the</strong> person is experiencing<br />

or doing, insufficient for empathizing with that person (this is what<br />

Goldie, 1999, calls “in his shoes imagining”). Consider, for example, <strong>the</strong> two<br />

characters described in <strong>the</strong> experiment of Kahneman and Tversky (1982).<br />

<strong>The</strong>se two persons arrive independently at <strong>the</strong> airport 30 minutes late. To<br />

one of <strong>the</strong>m, one explains that his plane left 30 minutes ago; one tells <strong>the</strong><br />

o<strong>the</strong>r that his plane was delayed and left only 5 minutes ago, too late for<br />

him to catch it. <strong>The</strong> experiment consists in asking subjects which of <strong>the</strong> two<br />

characters would be more frustrated. <strong>The</strong> response of most subjects is that<br />

<strong>the</strong> second one will be more frustrated. As <strong>the</strong>re is no rationale for that answer,<br />

<strong>the</strong> way subjects proceed to give it is to place <strong>the</strong>mselves in <strong>the</strong> shoes<br />

of <strong>the</strong> person and to feel what it would be like to miss a plane by only<br />

5 minutes. Obviously, subjects would give more circumstantial answers if<br />

<strong>the</strong>y could empathize more fully with <strong>the</strong> characters, that is, if <strong>the</strong>y knew

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!