12.12.2012 Views

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

Who Needs Emotions? The Brain Meets the Robot

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

eware <strong>the</strong> passionate robot 347<br />

However, this is only <strong>the</strong> first approximation in unraveling <strong>the</strong> circuits which<br />

enable <strong>the</strong> frog to tell predator from prey. Where Lettvin’s group emphasized<br />

retinal fly and enemy detectors, later work emphasized tectal integration<br />

(Grüsser-Cornehls & Grüsser, 1976) and interactive processes involving<br />

<strong>the</strong> optic tectum and <strong>the</strong> thalamic pretectal region (Ewert, 1987). Cobas<br />

and Arbib (1992) defined <strong>the</strong> perceptual and motor schemas involved in prey<br />

catching and predator avoidance in frog and toad, charting how differential<br />

activity in <strong>the</strong> tectum and pretectum could play upon midbrain mechanisms<br />

to activate <strong>the</strong> appropriate motor schemas:<br />

Prey capture: orient toward prey, advance, snap, consume<br />

Predator avoidance: orient away from predator, advance<br />

Note that <strong>the</strong> former includes “special-purpose” motor pattern generators,<br />

those for snapping and ingestion, while <strong>the</strong> latter uses only “general-purpose”<br />

motor pattern generators for turning and locomotion.<br />

Generic Feature Detectors in Cat Primary Visual Cortex<br />

In 1959, Hubel and Wiesel published “Receptive fields of single neurones in<br />

<strong>the</strong> cat’s striate cortex.” A whole string of fur<strong>the</strong>r papers (such as Hubel &<br />

Wiesel, 1962, 1965, 1968; Wiesel & Hubel, 1963; Hubel, Wiesel, & LeVay,<br />

1977) extended <strong>the</strong> story from cat to monkey, placed <strong>the</strong> neurophysiology<br />

in an anatomical and developmental framework, and introduced <strong>the</strong> crucial<br />

notions of orientation and ocular dominance columns in visual cortex—a<br />

cumulative achievement honored with a Nobel Prize in 1981. Where Kuffler<br />

(1953) had characterized retinal ganglion cells in cat as on-center offsurround<br />

and off-center on-surround, Hubel and Wiesel showed that cells<br />

in <strong>the</strong> primary visual cortex of cat (and monkey) could be classified as “simple”<br />

cortical cells, responsive to edges at a specific orientation in a specific place,<br />

and “complex” cells, which respond to edges of a given orientation in varying<br />

locations. Paralleling <strong>the</strong> work of Mountcastle and Powell (1959) on<br />

somatosensory cortex, Hubel and Wiesel found that <strong>the</strong> basic unit of mammalian<br />

visual cortex is <strong>the</strong> hypercolumn, 1 mm 2 × 2 mm deep. Each such<br />

hypercolumn contains columns responsive to specific orientations. <strong>The</strong> columns<br />

form an overarching retinotopic map, with fine-grained details such<br />

as orientation available as a “local tag” at each point of <strong>the</strong> map. Overlaid on<br />

this is <strong>the</strong> pattern of ocular dominance “columns” (really more like zebra<br />

stripes when viewed across <strong>the</strong> cortical surface), alternate bands each dominated<br />

by input from a single eye.<br />

How are we to reconcile <strong>the</strong> “ecologically significant” features extracted<br />

by <strong>the</strong> frog retina with <strong>the</strong> far more generic features seen in cats and primates

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!