02.05.2013 Views

Evolution__3rd_Edition

Evolution__3rd_Edition

Evolution__3rd_Edition

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

..<br />

Selfish<br />

The theories of group selection and<br />

of species selection are distinct<br />

Group selection can be<br />

experimentally simulated<br />

Empty<br />

Altruist<br />

CHAPTER 11 / The Units of Selection 303<br />

Figure 11.1<br />

Maynard Smith’s formulation of group selection models. A patch<br />

may change state in the direction of the arrows. Redrawn, by<br />

permission of the publisher, from Maynard Smith (1976). © 1976<br />

University of Chicago Press.<br />

its existence for the selfish trait to take over. This is a small number. So small that we<br />

can expect selfish individual adaptations to prevail in nature. Group selection, we<br />

conclude, is a weak force. It only works if migration rates are implausibly low and<br />

group extinction rates implausibly high. It is also not needed to explain the facts.<br />

The case against group selection is presented here in stark terms, but only to make<br />

the arguments clear. The matter has not been settled finally, and group selection probably<br />

operates sometimes. Moreover, group selection can have evolutionary consequences<br />

even if it never overrides individual selection. In Section 23.6 (p. 658) we look<br />

at a process called “species selection.” Species selection operates when different species<br />

(or even higher taxa) possess different individual-level adaptations, and their different<br />

adaptations have different consequences for the rate of extinction or speciation. Taxa<br />

with lower extinction, or higher speciation, rates tend to proliferate. Much the same<br />

could be true of groups within a species.<br />

In species selection, there is no conflict between selection at lower (individual) and<br />

higher (species, or even group) levels. In all the species (or groups), individuals act in<br />

their own selfish interest. Species selection is theoretically uncontroversial, though its<br />

empirical importance is open to doubt. The controversy about group selection that we<br />

looked at above was theoretical as well as empirical. Critics of group selection doubt<br />

whether group selection could be strong enough to cause individuals to sacrifice their<br />

own reproductive interests to those of its group.<br />

In nature, group selection is rarely likely to override individual selection, and to<br />

establish individually disadvantageous behavior. In the laboratory, however, conditions<br />

can be made extreme enough for it to do so. Let us finish by looking at such a<br />

case: Wade’s experiment on flour beetles, Tribolium castaneum. The life cycle of the<br />

flour beetle takes place, from egg to adult, in stored flour. They are pests, but they have<br />

also become a population biologist’s standard experimental animal, particularly in<br />

Chicago. Wade set up an experiment to illustrate Wynne-Edwards’ hypothesis of<br />

reproductive restraint.<br />

Figure 11.2a illustrates the experimental design. In each of three experimental treatments,<br />

there were 48 different colonies of Tribolium. Each colony was allowed to breed<br />

for 37 days. Then 48 new colonies, of 16 beetles each, were set up from the progeny of<br />

the old ones. Wade (1976) artificially selected for groups that had showed a low (or<br />

high) fecundity (the third treatment was a control). He selected for low fecundity by<br />

forming a new generation of colonies from Tribolium colonies that had a low population<br />

density at the end of the 37 days; and for high fecundity by forming each new<br />

generation from colonies that had high population densities. He repeated a number of<br />

rounds of the process.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!