02.05.2013 Views

Evolution__3rd_Edition

Evolution__3rd_Edition

Evolution__3rd_Edition

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

384 PART 4 / <strong>Evolution</strong> and Diversity<br />

Populations can be kept on<br />

different resources ...<br />

. . . and prezygotic isolation evolves<br />

between them<br />

The result is general, and<br />

remarkable<br />

14.3.1 Laboratory experiments illustrate how separately evolving<br />

populations of a species tend incidentally to evolve<br />

reproductive isolation<br />

When two geographically separate populations are evolving independently, different<br />

genes will be fixed in each, whether by drift or adaptation to different environments.<br />

The theory of allopatric speciation suggests that two such populations will also, at least<br />

sometimes, evolve some degree of reproductive isolation in consequence.<br />

The idea has been tested experimentally. We can keep two populations apart, allowing<br />

them to evolve indendently for a number of generations. Then we test whether they<br />

have evolved any degree of reproductive isolation. Dodd (1989), for example, performed<br />

the experiment with fruitflies (Drosophila pseudoobscura). The flies had originally<br />

been caught in Utah and were then taken to a lab at Yale and divided into eight<br />

populations: four of them were placed on a starch-based food medium; the other four<br />

on a maltose-based medium. The populations were reared on these different resources<br />

for a number of generations. After a while the flies had evolved detectable differences in<br />

their digestive enzymes a differences that were almost certainly adaptations to the different<br />

resources. Thus, the populations had diverged under the influence of selection to<br />

live on different resources in the laboratory.<br />

Dodd exploited these populations to see whether any reproductive isolation had<br />

evolved as an incidental consequence of the divergence. She placed recently emerged<br />

males and females from the starch and maltose populations in a cage, after marking all<br />

the individuals of one of the populations. She then measured who mated with whom,<br />

and found that the “starch” flies preferred a “starch” mate, and the “maltose” flies a<br />

“maltose” mate (Figure 14.2). Some reproductive isolation had evolved a in this case it<br />

is prezygotic isolation. It presumably evolved because the changes that had occurred in<br />

the population influenced reproductive behavior in some way.<br />

Dodd’s is only one experiment among many. Rice & Hostert (1993) listed 14 experiments<br />

that measured whether prezygotic isolation emerged between populations that<br />

had been experimentally isolated, and found that in 11 of them it did; in the other three<br />

there was no significant change. The evolution of reproductive isolation is a general<br />

result in experiments in which two populations are evolving separately, in different<br />

environmental conditions. The result is quite striking because it might not have been<br />

predicted. Would you expect that if you kept one population of humans on a starch diet<br />

and another on a sugar diet for a number of generations, that at the end of the period<br />

they would have evolved a mating preference for their own dietic type? The evolution<br />

of adaptations to the food supply (starch in one population, sugar in the other) is<br />

predictable. The evolution of reproductive isolation might not be, but it does seem to<br />

happen. The result is also interesting because reproductive isolation is not, at least<br />

directly, being selected for. The adaptations to the environment (such as diet) though<br />

are being selected for, and the flies in Dodd’s experiment duly evolved appropriate<br />

digestive enzymes. However, the reproductive isolation just “drops out,” as an incidental<br />

consequence of the experimental procedure. The experiment did not selectively breed<br />

from individuals that showed some mating preference, such that mating preferences<br />

evolved upwards over the generations. The mating preference somehow evolves as a<br />

correlated response when selection favors new adaptations to the environment.<br />

..

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!