27.06.2013 Views

Gibson Ferguson Language Planning and Education Edinburgh ...

Gibson Ferguson Language Planning and Education Edinburgh ...

Gibson Ferguson Language Planning and Education Edinburgh ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

52 <strong>Language</strong> <strong>Planning</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Education</strong><br />

3.2.1.2 The Rossell <strong>and</strong> Baker (1996) <strong>and</strong> Greene (1997) studies<br />

Focusing – somewhat narrowly – on a comparison of TBE with four other program<br />

types (submersion, ESL, ‘structured immersion’ <strong>and</strong> DBE) with respect to outcomes<br />

in English language, reading in English <strong>and</strong> mathematics, Rossell <strong>and</strong> Baker<br />

reviewed over 300 studies, identifying only 72 (25 per cent) as methodologically<br />

acceptable. 17 The key findings were:<br />

• On st<strong>and</strong>ardised tests TBE proved superior to submersion in only 22 per cent<br />

of studies in the case of reading, 7 per cent in English language, <strong>and</strong> 9 per cent<br />

in mathematics.<br />

• In none of the studies comparing TBE with ‘structured immersion’ (N=12) was<br />

there any advantage for TBE in reading, English language or mathematics.<br />

• In the one study comparing TBE with maintenance bilingual education there<br />

was an advantage in reading skills for the TBE program.<br />

Rossell <strong>and</strong> Baker (1996: 44) conclude that ‘the case for transitional bilingual<br />

education is not based on the soundly derived research evidence that its supporters<br />

claim’. Their preferred program type is English-only ‘structured immersion’,<br />

‘modelled after the Canadian immersion programs’, though they concede that<br />

that type of instructional program is only one of many factors influencing school<br />

achievement.<br />

While these findings are at first sight very unfavourable to bilingual education,<br />

closer scrutiny of the Rossell <strong>and</strong> Baker (1996) methodology has revealed flaws that<br />

substantially undermine their validity. These include:<br />

1. The use of narrative review, <strong>and</strong> in particular the raw, unsophisticated ‘votecounting’<br />

technique, which tallies studies for <strong>and</strong> against TBE. The problem<br />

here is that, given variations between studies in sample size, power <strong>and</strong> design,<br />

it is not at all clear that they should be equally weighted.<br />

2. Programs are compared by label with little or no exploration of their actual<br />

operationalisation (e.g. Crawford 1997). 18<br />

3. Rossell <strong>and</strong> Baker (1996) appear to equate Canadian immersion programs<br />

with ‘structured immersion’ programs in the United States, drawing on one as<br />

evidence for the other, when in fact, as pointed out earlier, the two have quite<br />

different goals <strong>and</strong> serve quite different student populations (Cummins 1998,<br />

2000).<br />

4. Criteria for methodologically acceptable studies are applied inconsistently,<br />

resulting in the inclusion of some studies that should by these same criteria<br />

have been excluded (Greene 1997: 4).<br />

There are good reasons, then, for concluding that the Rossell <strong>and</strong> Baker review is<br />

flawed, <strong>and</strong> cannot bear the weight placed on it by critics of BE in public discussion<br />

of Title VII programs.<br />

In a curious parallel to Willig’s re-analysis of the Baker <strong>and</strong> De Kanter (1981)<br />

review, Greene (1997) re-examined the 72 studies reviewed by Rossell <strong>and</strong> Baker,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!