27.06.2013 Views

Gibson Ferguson Language Planning and Education Edinburgh ...

Gibson Ferguson Language Planning and Education Edinburgh ...

Gibson Ferguson Language Planning and Education Edinburgh ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Education</strong>al <strong>and</strong> political dimensions of bilingual education 55<br />

Underlying this m<strong>and</strong>ate is an educational rationale based on three key principles,<br />

the first of which is the so-called ‘time-on-task’ principle, most explicitly propounded<br />

by Porter (1990), which claims a direct, positive correlation between the<br />

amount of time spent in learning a second language <strong>and</strong> proficiency in that language.<br />

Neatly dovetailing with popular intuition, the principle would suggest that if<br />

children need to learn English to access opportunities, then they should be given<br />

as much classroom instruction in, <strong>and</strong> through, English as possible. Instruction<br />

through the pupil’s native language is nothing but an impediment to the acquisition<br />

of English, <strong>and</strong> thereby to LEP pupils’ academic attainment.<br />

The second principle, more a notion actually, <strong>and</strong> implied more than explicit,<br />

is that instruction in two languages unduly burdens pupils’ cognitive capacities,<br />

especially average pupils, <strong>and</strong> can adversely affect educational performance. It would<br />

be more effective, then, to concentrate effort on developing pupils’ skills in English,<br />

the language of opportunity.<br />

The third principle is that younger children learn languages readily <strong>and</strong> rapidly,<br />

<strong>and</strong>, on the whole, are better learners than older children. The policy implication<br />

derived is that English teaching – in the form of structured English immersion –<br />

should start as early as possible. The point is made with unusual explicitness in<br />

Article 1 of Proposition 227:<br />

(e) Whereas young immigrant children can easily acquire fluency in a new<br />

language, such as English, if they are heavily exposed to that language in the<br />

classroom at an early age.<br />

(f ) Therefore it is resolved that: all children in California public schools shall be<br />

taught English as rapidly <strong>and</strong> effectively as possible.<br />

We now discuss each of these principles in reverse order.<br />

3.2.3.1 Principles 3: ‘The younger, the better’<br />

Extended discussion of the third principle (the optimal age question in Second<br />

<strong>Language</strong> Acquisition (SLA)) lies beyond the scope of this chapter, so we confine<br />

ourselves to the straightforward point (Scovel 2000a, 2000b) that older learners may<br />

be advantaged in certain areas (e.g. vocabulary learning, pragmatics, literacy) <strong>and</strong><br />

younger learners in others (e.g. pronunciation, syntax). In addition, age is not a<br />

‘st<strong>and</strong>-alone’ factor but interacts with a range of other contextual <strong>and</strong> developmental<br />

factors jointly influencing second language acquisition. In a policy-relevant review,<br />

Johnstone (2002: 20) concludes on a positive note that:<br />

given suitable teaching, motivation, <strong>and</strong> support, it is possible to make a success<br />

of language learning at any age <strong>and</strong> stage, though older learners are less likely to<br />

approximate to the levels of a native speaker.<br />

The evidence for what Scovel (2000a: 114) calls ‘the younger, the better myth’ is thus<br />

more equivocal than the Proposition 227 drafters suggest.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!