13.07.2015 Views

Reaching the marginalized: EFA global monitoring report, 2010; 2010

Reaching the marginalized: EFA global monitoring report, 2010; 2010

Reaching the marginalized: EFA global monitoring report, 2010; 2010

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

100CHAPTER 32Education for All Global Monitoring Report8. In three test rounds,scores for low-castechildren fell by 14%,25% and 39%.Figure 3.24: In India, scheduled castes and tribes remaindisadvantaged at all levels in educationAttendance rates by age group in India, by community, rural/urban residenceand gender, 2004/2005School attendance rate (%)10080604020National averageScheduled casteScheduled caste girls, ruralScheduled tribe6–11 12–14 15–18Age groupScheduled tribe girls, ruralNotes: The attendance rate for an age range is <strong>the</strong> proportion of children of that agerange who <strong>report</strong> attending school at <strong>the</strong> time of <strong>the</strong> survey. The age ranges correspondapproximately to primary education, upper primary (or ‘middle’) education and secondaryeducation, respectively, in <strong>the</strong> Indian school system.Source: Bhalotra (2009) based on National Sample Survey data (61st round).To what extent do <strong>the</strong>se differences stem fromdistinctive caste and tribe disadvantages ra<strong>the</strong>rthan wider social and economic factors? That is akey question for policy-makers seeking to equalizeopportunity. Research for this Report helps providea partial answer (Bhalotra, 2009). Using householdsurvey data, and controlling for household andindividual characteristics, <strong>the</strong> study found thatabout 60% of <strong>the</strong> attendance gap for scheduledcastechildren aged 6 to 14 could be attributed tohousehold characteristics, mainly poverty and lowerparental education. For scheduled-tribe childrenin <strong>the</strong> same age group, household characteristicsweighed less heavily, accounting for about 40% of<strong>the</strong> attendance gap. One conclusion to be drawn formembers of both scheduled groups is that povertymatters a great deal in perpetuating educationaldisadvantage. However, <strong>the</strong> non-poverty componentis larger for scheduled tribes partly because of <strong>the</strong>weight of social and cultural discrimination.Public attitudes have consequences that go beyondschool attendance. Institutionalized stigmatizationcan erode self-confidence and levels of expectation,undermining children’s potential for learning.One particularly striking illustration comes from anexperimental investigation into <strong>the</strong> impact of casteperceptions on test scores (Hoff and Pandey, 2004).Children aged 11 and 12 were chosen at randomfrom a low caste and three high castes, and givena series of puzzles to solve. When caste was notannounced to <strong>the</strong> participants, it had no bearingon <strong>the</strong> initial score or on <strong>the</strong> improvement in scoreregistered in subsequent test rounds. But whencaste was announced before <strong>the</strong> test, <strong>the</strong> scores forlow-caste children fell dramatically (Figure 3.25). 8These findings underline <strong>the</strong> degree to whichsocial identities that are a product of history,culture and personal experience can createpronounced education disadvantages through<strong>the</strong>ir effects on individual expectations.The critical role of languageLanguage and ethnicity are deeply intertwined.Having a distinctive language is often a crucialelement of personal identity and group attachment.Just as a local language may be a point ofassociation for members of an ethnic group, itcan also be an element in <strong>the</strong>ir marginalization.People who cannot speak a country’s dominantlanguage may have less access to written andspoken sources, restricting <strong>the</strong>ir opportunitiesfor employment and social mobility (Smits andGündüz-Hosgör, 2003; Smits et al., 2008). Parentswho do not speak <strong>the</strong> official language in which<strong>the</strong>ir children are being educated may have lessopportunity to engage with teachers, educationauthorities and homework. And <strong>the</strong>ir childrenmay not grasp what is being taught if teachersdo not speak <strong>the</strong>ir home language. The resultinginequalities in opportunity are a major factorFigure 3.25: Social stigma can undermine test performanceExperimental impact of <strong>the</strong> announcement of caste on solvingpuzzles in IndiaAverage number of puzzles solved43210Caste not announcedCaste announcedHigh casteLow casteNote: Children aged 11 and 12 were given a packet of fifteen maze puzzlesand asked to solve as many as <strong>the</strong>y could in fifteen minutes.Source: Hoff and Pandey (2004).172

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!