16.11.2012 Views

Biomechanics and Medicine in Swimming XI

Biomechanics and Medicine in Swimming XI

Biomechanics and Medicine in Swimming XI

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

legs than they could float motionless on their front or back. Despite<br />

Harrod’s f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, the Red Cross never did <strong>in</strong>troduce glid<strong>in</strong>g skills prior<br />

to float<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> their learn-to-swim program (American Red Cross, 1992).<br />

At least with adults, it appears that float<strong>in</strong>g probably can be taught prior<br />

to <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>g the glid<strong>in</strong>g skills. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, the two glid<strong>in</strong>g skill items<br />

were as difficult for the current sample to perform as were the elementary<br />

backstroke <strong>and</strong> sidestroke. This difficulty is somewhat surpris<strong>in</strong>g<br />

s<strong>in</strong>ce both front <strong>and</strong> back glid<strong>in</strong>g nom<strong>in</strong>ally would appear to be much<br />

less complicated <strong>and</strong> difficult items than the two rest<strong>in</strong>g strokes.<br />

Based upon these results, the authors could not suggest to the American<br />

Red Cross that there was any evidence-based support to alter the<br />

order <strong>in</strong> which the thirteen selected skill items ought to be presented<br />

<strong>in</strong> the revised 2009 American Red Cross learn-to-swim program. The<br />

study obviously did not test all pert<strong>in</strong>ent skill items <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the Red<br />

Cross’ learn-to-swim program <strong>and</strong> therefore the results should not be<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpreted to apply to the validity of the order for skills <strong>in</strong> the entire<br />

learn-to-swim program. Because the current sample was limited to a relatively<br />

small convenience sample of normally-abled young adult college<br />

students, it also is not possible to suggest that even the order of these<br />

items with the strong coefficient of reproducibility actually applies to<br />

other <strong>in</strong>dividuals such as preschool or elementary-age children or to <strong>in</strong>dividuals<br />

who may be differently-abled. Samples drawn from other specific<br />

populations would be required to underst<strong>and</strong> whether this order of<br />

acquisition, <strong>and</strong> by extension, <strong>in</strong>structional order, should be considered<br />

to be universal or whether the order is unique to different age <strong>and</strong> ability<br />

groups. Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g how much variability <strong>in</strong> order is associated with<br />

different ages <strong>and</strong> ability groups would be crucial pieces of <strong>in</strong>formation<br />

to support future evidence-based revisions to learn-to-swim programs<br />

such as the American Red Cross.<br />

The study <strong>and</strong> its results have some very def<strong>in</strong>ite limitations. A sample<br />

size of thirty-one was relatively small which limited the statistical<br />

power with which the descriptive scalogram could identify variability<br />

<strong>in</strong> the order of item difficulty. We <strong>in</strong>itially had <strong>in</strong>tended to test all sixty<br />

class participants, but some students did not volunteer while others were<br />

absent some days <strong>and</strong> did not complete all items. The sample was def<strong>in</strong>itely<br />

a convenience sample which meant that some of the less skilled<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals opted not to participate, skew<strong>in</strong>g the participant skill levels<br />

as evidenced by the high average number of successful item completions.<br />

A scalogram relies upon test<strong>in</strong>g a truly heterogeneous sample that<br />

should <strong>in</strong>clude persons who are capable of pass<strong>in</strong>g few if any of the<br />

items. The current sample of young adults was def<strong>in</strong>itely skewed toward<br />

a more homogeneous <strong>and</strong> skilled sample which is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g given<br />

that they were recruited from <strong>in</strong>structional swimm<strong>in</strong>g classes of college<br />

students.<br />

Because the test<strong>in</strong>g occurred dur<strong>in</strong>g the middle <strong>in</strong>stead of the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of the semester of study, it is probable that previous <strong>in</strong>struction<br />

biased the sample toward greater skillfulness. It also appears that the<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual swim <strong>in</strong>structor who was an assistant swim coach emphasized<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> practice of the competitive swim strokes of front <strong>and</strong> back<br />

crawl, breaststroke, <strong>and</strong> butterfly <strong>and</strong> not rest<strong>in</strong>g strokes of elementary<br />

backstroke <strong>and</strong> sidestroke. Presumably this expla<strong>in</strong>s the lower success<br />

rate observed <strong>in</strong> performance of the elementary backstroke <strong>and</strong> sidestroke<br />

items. Many students appeared puzzled <strong>and</strong> had never heard of<br />

these two strokes when presented with them as test items <strong>in</strong> the study.<br />

The <strong>in</strong>structional bias also presents a challenge to the potential utility<br />

of the scalogram <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the robustness of the coefficient<br />

of reproducibility. It was generally presumed by Guttman (1950)<br />

that item difficulty possessed some <strong>in</strong>herently <strong>in</strong>variant task qualities<br />

<strong>and</strong> characteristics not substantively <strong>in</strong>fluenced by <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>struction.<br />

In the current sample, it appeared fairly clear that familiarity<br />

with competitive strokes <strong>and</strong> lack of familiarity with rest<strong>in</strong>g strokes<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluenced the degree of success demonstrated by some participants.<br />

This def<strong>in</strong>itely could have <strong>in</strong>fluenced the order of item acquisition. The<br />

authors have personal <strong>in</strong>structional experience that both the elementary<br />

backstroke <strong>and</strong> sidestroke can be effectively <strong>in</strong>troduced prior to front<br />

chaPter5.education,advice<strong>and</strong>BiofeedBack<br />

<strong>and</strong> back crawl strokes. The current results tend to negate those personal<br />

observations.<br />

The observation of potential <strong>in</strong>struction or <strong>in</strong>tervention bias means<br />

that further studies us<strong>in</strong>g a scalogram should be conducted under experimental<br />

conditions. Samples with well def<strong>in</strong>ed differences <strong>in</strong> experiences<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>struction need to be compared to clarify the degree to which the<br />

coefficient of reproducibility is altered. For example if the first author<br />

<strong>and</strong> the assistant coach <strong>in</strong>structor of the classes <strong>in</strong> the current study<br />

both taught similar groups of young adults, one emphasiz<strong>in</strong>g acquisition<br />

of rest<strong>in</strong>g strokes <strong>and</strong> the other focus<strong>in</strong>g on the competitive strokes,<br />

would different orders <strong>and</strong> coefficients of reproducibility result? There<br />

are def<strong>in</strong>itely pedagogical <strong>and</strong> methodological questions that should <strong>and</strong><br />

could be addressed.<br />

conclusIon<br />

The current study employed Guttman’s (1950) descriptive scalogram<br />

technique to exam<strong>in</strong>e the validity of swim skill item order of acquisition<br />

<strong>in</strong> a small convenience sample of young adults. The <strong>in</strong>structional order of<br />

thirteen swimm<strong>in</strong>g skills used by the 2004 American Red Cross learnto-swim<br />

program was tested us<strong>in</strong>g a scalogram. The Red Cross order<br />

of the thirteen swimm<strong>in</strong>g items produced a very strong coefficient of<br />

reproducibility (CR = 0.93) <strong>and</strong> provided some evidence-based support<br />

for the Red Cross to cont<strong>in</strong>ue present<strong>in</strong>g those swimm<strong>in</strong>g items <strong>in</strong> a<br />

similar order <strong>in</strong> their learn-to-swim programs. The results of the study<br />

revealed several limitations <strong>in</strong> the conduct of the study (e.g., relatively<br />

small convenience sample, limited skill <strong>and</strong> age range of participants) as<br />

well as raised questions about the robustness of the scalogram technique<br />

itself <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the coefficient of reproducibility. The authors<br />

have proposed several subsequent studies to clarify the technique<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the results.<br />

reFerences<br />

American National Red Cross. (1981). Swimm<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> aquatics safety.<br />

Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, D.C.<br />

American Red Cross. (1992). Water Safety Instructor Manual. St. Louis:<br />

Mosby Year Book.<br />

American Red Cross. (2004). Water Safety Instructor’s Manual. Yardley,<br />

PA: StayWell.<br />

American Red Cross. (2009). Water Safety Instructor’s Manual (r.09).<br />

Yardley, PA: StayWell.<br />

Darw<strong>in</strong>, C. (1877, July). A biographical sketch of an <strong>in</strong>fant. M<strong>in</strong>d, 285-<br />

294.<br />

DeOreo, K. (1976). Dynamic balance <strong>in</strong> preschool children: Quantify<strong>in</strong>g<br />

qualitative data. Research Quarterly, 47, 526-531.<br />

Erbaugh, S.J. (1978). Assessment of swimm<strong>in</strong>g performance of preschool<br />

children. Perceptual <strong>and</strong> Motor Skills, 47, 1179-1182.<br />

Erbaugh, S. (1980). The development of swimm<strong>in</strong>g skills of preschool<br />

children. In C. Nadeau, K. Newell, G. Roberts, & W. Halliwell (Eds.),<br />

Psychology of motor behavior <strong>and</strong> sport-1979 (pp. 324-335). Champaign,<br />

IL: Human K<strong>in</strong>etics.<br />

Gesell, A. (1940). The first five years of life. New York: Harper <strong>and</strong> Row.<br />

Guttman, L.L. (1950). The basis for scalogram analysis. In S.A. Stouffer,<br />

L. Guttman, E. Suchman, P. Lazerfeld, S. Star, & J. Clausen (Eds.),<br />

Measurement <strong>and</strong> prediction (pp. 60-90). Pr<strong>in</strong>ceton, NJ: Pr<strong>in</strong>ceton<br />

University Press.<br />

Harrod, D. (1990). A scalogram analysis of the American Red Cross Beg<strong>in</strong>ner<br />

swimm<strong>in</strong>g skill items. Unpublished Master’s thesis, Kent State<br />

University, Kent, OH.<br />

Harrod, D., & Langendorfer, S.J. (1991). A scalogram analysis of American<br />

Red Cross Beg<strong>in</strong>ner swimm<strong>in</strong>g skill items. National Aquatics<br />

Journal, 6, 10-16.<br />

Haywood, K.M., & Getchell, N. (2009). LifeSpan Motor Development<br />

(5 th Ed.). Champaign, IL: Human K<strong>in</strong>etics.<br />

Herkowitz, J. (1978). Developmental task analysis: The design of movement<br />

experiences <strong>and</strong> evaluation of motor development status. In<br />

335

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!