06.02.2013 Views

Research in Engineering Education Symposium 2011 - rees2009

Research in Engineering Education Symposium 2011 - rees2009

Research in Engineering Education Symposium 2011 - rees2009

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Universidad Politécnica de Madrid (UPM) Pág<strong>in</strong>a 301 de 957<br />

Theoretical Framework<br />

Variation Theory<br />

Developed from phenomenography , Variation Theory (Marton & Booth, 1997; Marton &<br />

Tsui, 2004; Marton & Pang, 2006; Pang & Marton, 2005) views conceptions <strong>in</strong> terms of<br />

awareness, and learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> terms of changes <strong>in</strong> the learner’s awareness structure as it<br />

relates to a concept or a phenomenon <strong>in</strong> general. That is, different conceptions correspond<br />

to awareness at different levels of complexity that are derived from different ways of<br />

experienc<strong>in</strong>g a phenomenon. The variation between conceptions is due to the different<br />

aspects of the phenomenon that the learner is aware of, or is able to discern when he or<br />

she experiences the phenomenon. The variation <strong>in</strong> learners’ awareness or how learners<br />

experience a phenomenon can be described <strong>in</strong> terms of aspects or dimensions of variation<br />

–“the different ways of experienc<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g are differtent ways of experienc<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

same th<strong>in</strong>g, the variation <strong>in</strong> ways of experienc<strong>in</strong>g the same th<strong>in</strong>g, the variation <strong>in</strong> ways of<br />

experienc<strong>in</strong>g it can be describe <strong>in</strong> terms of a set of dimensions of variation” (Marton &<br />

Booth, 1997, p.108). Learn<strong>in</strong>g is considered to take place when students become aware of<br />

the aspects of variation that they have not been able to discern previously.<br />

Aimed at describ<strong>in</strong>g and expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g variation with<strong>in</strong> learners’ conceptions or experiences,<br />

Variation Theory usually leads to “an outcome space” that consists of a hierarchical set of<br />

<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly more sophisticated categories of conception or understand<strong>in</strong>g. This structure,<br />

together with its empirically driven nature, makes Variation Theory an appropriate choice<br />

to guide the development of learn<strong>in</strong>g progressions. Most importantly, it po<strong>in</strong>ts to a<br />

systematic way of characteriz<strong>in</strong>g the different conceptions that students exhibit, namely,<br />

to identify the critical aspects of variation that set their understand<strong>in</strong>g apart from each<br />

other.<br />

Methodology<br />

To understand students´conceptions of “size and scale”, we conducted a series of three<br />

studies. As the focus of this paper is not on the studies themselves, but the use of Variation<br />

Theory <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the results, only essential details of the studies are provided here.<br />

Further <strong>in</strong>formation can be found elsewhere (Light et al., 2007, 2008; Swarat et al., <strong>2011</strong>).<br />

Participants were undergraduate students enrolled <strong>in</strong> a first-year eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g course and<br />

a non-major materials science course at a major Midwest university <strong>in</strong> the USA. Both<br />

courses <strong>in</strong>cluded units focused on nanoscience. The first study was more exploratory <strong>in</strong><br />

nature. It <strong>in</strong>volved 12 students from diverse backgrounds <strong>in</strong> task-based, th<strong>in</strong>k-aloud<br />

<strong>in</strong>terviews. Students were asked to order a list of objects of widely vary<strong>in</strong>g sizes (football<br />

field, elephant, typical science textbook, human hair, bacterium, virus, and atom) along a<br />

l<strong>in</strong>e, and then to apply a numerical scale to represent their size differences. The second<br />

study used a similar methodological approach, but the <strong>in</strong>terview focus was narrowed.<br />

Students (n=20) were given scale examples generated <strong>in</strong> the first round of <strong>in</strong>terviews, and<br />

asked to evaluate their appropriateness. The third study adm<strong>in</strong>istered a set of assessment<br />

items <strong>in</strong> the form of multiple choice and short answer to 111 students. These items were<br />

Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Education</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2011</strong><br />

Madrid, 4 th - 7 th October <strong>2011</strong>

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!