06.02.2013 Views

Research in Engineering Education Symposium 2011 - rees2009

Research in Engineering Education Symposium 2011 - rees2009

Research in Engineering Education Symposium 2011 - rees2009

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Universidad Politécnica de Madrid (UPM) Pág<strong>in</strong>a 335 de 957<br />

groups. Nicol & Boyle (2003) compare two published collaborative learn<strong>in</strong>g methods<br />

(peer <strong>in</strong>struction vs. class-wide discussion). Although group size was not the only<br />

difference between these two methods it was a significant variable and students felt that<br />

the small group discussion was more effective than the class-wide discussion method.<br />

Individual pre-discussion work required<br />

In the study reported <strong>in</strong> this paper most of the collaborative learn<strong>in</strong>g processes with<br />

significant positive benefits required students to answer some relevant questions<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividually before the group discussion occurred. However, three of the eight did not<br />

report whether <strong>in</strong>dividual pre-discussion work was required or not. Amongst the neutral<br />

studies three of the five required <strong>in</strong>dividual pre-discussion work, one did not and one did<br />

not report whether it was required or not. So while most studies seem to suggest<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual pre-discussion work is useful, it is not the only factor contribut<strong>in</strong>g to the<br />

success of the activity s<strong>in</strong>ce some neutral studies <strong>in</strong>cluded it too.<br />

The other significant variable between the two peer learn<strong>in</strong>g methods reported by Nicol &<br />

Boyle (2003), apart from the group size as discussed above, is that with the peer<br />

<strong>in</strong>struction method students were required to answer the problems <strong>in</strong>dividually before<br />

engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> discussion with their group members about which was the correct answer and<br />

why. They found that: “Almost all the students <strong>in</strong>terviewed expressed a preference for<br />

start<strong>in</strong>g the concept test with <strong>in</strong>dividual th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and an <strong>in</strong>dividual response, rather than<br />

with peer discussion. They were forced to th<strong>in</strong>k about the problem, and to formulate their<br />

own reason for their selected answer before the group discussion. Hav<strong>in</strong>g constructed<br />

their own answer students felt they benefited more from the subsequent peer discussion.”<br />

Haller et al (2000) suggest a reason why gett<strong>in</strong>g students to <strong>in</strong>dividually attempt<br />

questions before the discussion can help the group function. They analysed the<br />

conversation of four groups as they solved group homework problems and identified two<br />

types of conversation sequences: transfer of knowledge sequence (teacher/pupil<br />

relationship) and the collaborative sequence (shared th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g out loud to achieve jo<strong>in</strong>t<br />

understand<strong>in</strong>g). Furthermore they found that <strong>in</strong>teraction patterns referred to as ‘constant<br />

pupil’ and the ‘blocker’ tended to <strong>in</strong>terfere with group function. In the constant pupil<br />

<strong>in</strong>teraction the same group member needs to have concepts expla<strong>in</strong>ed by other members<br />

of the group which can lead to these members feel<strong>in</strong>g slowed down or that the constant<br />

pupil is not pull<strong>in</strong>g their weight. From this research these authors recommend that<br />

students should attempt the homework problems alone before the group meet<strong>in</strong>g as this<br />

would m<strong>in</strong>imise the constant pupil type of <strong>in</strong>teraction.<br />

Formative or summative activity<br />

Ramsden (1992) tells us that students experience our courses through the assessment<br />

tasks and allocate their time depend<strong>in</strong>g on what activities will earn them marks.<br />

Academics have often <strong>in</strong>terpreted this as mean<strong>in</strong>g they have to allocate marks to an<br />

activity to get students to do it and that the marks allocated reflect the value of the<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g we expect students to get out of that activity.<br />

Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Education</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2011</strong><br />

Madrid, 4 th - 7 th October <strong>2011</strong>

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!