06.02.2013 Views

Research in Engineering Education Symposium 2011 - rees2009

Research in Engineering Education Symposium 2011 - rees2009

Research in Engineering Education Symposium 2011 - rees2009

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Universidad Politécnica de Madrid (UPM) Pág<strong>in</strong>a 493 de 957<br />

revolutions, and cognitive conflict will successfully lead students to the scientific theory or<br />

understand<strong>in</strong>g of a given phenomena.<br />

How do people react to anomalous data?<br />

Anomalous data is data that does not conform to a student’s present understand<strong>in</strong>g of a<br />

particular phenomenon (Ch<strong>in</strong>n & Brewer, 1993). Educators often assume that present<strong>in</strong>g<br />

a student with anomalous data will result <strong>in</strong> cognitive conflict that will lead him or her to<br />

reject or modify their naïve understand<strong>in</strong>g of a particular phenomenon <strong>in</strong> favour of an<br />

understand<strong>in</strong>g that is more closely aligned with accepted scientific explanations (Posner,<br />

Brewer, Strike & Herzog, 1982). However, this is often not the case. An excellent example<br />

is students attempt<strong>in</strong>g to transition from understand<strong>in</strong>g the Earth as flat to understand<strong>in</strong>g<br />

that it is a sphere (Vosniadou, 2003): Students f<strong>in</strong>d it difficult to reconcile their classroom<br />

<strong>in</strong>struction which states that the Earth is a sphere with their own experiences which<br />

suggest that the Earth is flat. Instead of reject<strong>in</strong>g the flat earth model, they <strong>in</strong>stead w<strong>in</strong>d up<br />

with a dual Earth model where there is one Earth that is a sphere and travels through the<br />

solar system, and a different flat Earth that they actually live on. Understand<strong>in</strong>gs built on<br />

personal experience do not change readily when students are exposed to anomalous data.<br />

Ch<strong>in</strong>n & Brewer (1993) developed a theoretical framework to expla<strong>in</strong> the wide variety of<br />

ways that students respond to anomalous data. They identified seven different<br />

psychological responses that students may have regard<strong>in</strong>g their naïve theory (call it<br />

Theory A) <strong>in</strong> response to anomalous data that supports a scientific theory (Theory B).<br />

Students may simply ignore the anomalous data, and forget about or ignore the data. They<br />

may also reject the data, which is similar to ignor<strong>in</strong>g the data except that they can provide<br />

a reason for reject<strong>in</strong>g the data. These reasons could <strong>in</strong>clude assum<strong>in</strong>g that there was a<br />

methodological error <strong>in</strong> generat<strong>in</strong>g the data, believ<strong>in</strong>g that the anomalous data are a fluke<br />

due to random variation <strong>in</strong> the data, or believ<strong>in</strong>g that the data are fraudulent. Students<br />

may also exclude the data from the doma<strong>in</strong> of Theory A, believ<strong>in</strong>g that their theory is not<br />

supposed to expla<strong>in</strong> the anomalous data. They may also hold the data <strong>in</strong> abeyance,<br />

acknowledg<strong>in</strong>g its validity but believ<strong>in</strong>g that their theory will be able to expla<strong>in</strong> the<br />

anomalous data <strong>in</strong> the future. Students can also re<strong>in</strong>terpret the data so that it fits better<br />

with Theory A. F<strong>in</strong>ally, the students actually engage <strong>in</strong> conceptual change and make<br />

peripheral changes to Theory A (weak conceptual change), or accept the data and change<br />

Theory A, possibly so that it more closely resembles Theory B (strong conceptual change).<br />

Students’ responses to anomalous data vary due to a variety of factors (Ch<strong>in</strong>n & Brewer,<br />

1993). Several characteristics of students’ prior knowledge have a strong effect on their<br />

responses to anomalous data. This <strong>in</strong>cludes the entrenchment of their current<br />

understand<strong>in</strong>g based on the amount of experience they have with the phenomenon <strong>in</strong><br />

question. The students’ ontological beliefs about the fundamental categories and<br />

properties of the world can be particularly difficult to change because they can support<br />

understand<strong>in</strong>g across doma<strong>in</strong>s. Their epistemological beliefs about what constitutes<br />

knowledge and sound theories can also affect their responses to anomalous data. The<br />

students’ background knowledge and familiarity with mathematical, scientific, or other<br />

concepts related to the theory <strong>in</strong> question can also affect how they react to anomalous<br />

Proceed<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Education</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> <strong>2011</strong><br />

Madrid, 4 th - 7 th October <strong>2011</strong>

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!