26.11.2012 Views

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

30<br />

(42) is pervasive <strong>and</strong> systematic, not jeopardized by narrowly-defined exceptions,<br />

<strong>and</strong> dem<strong>and</strong>s a principled explanation. 9<br />

The example in (43) illustrates both phonology-free syntax, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> essential<br />

difference between breaches <strong>of</strong> strong modularity <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> simple cumulation <strong>of</strong><br />

processing by multiple modules. A phonology-free syntax cannot access phonological<br />

information or operations, <strong>and</strong> phonology in turn cannot manipulate syntax.<br />

However, phonology may filter <strong>the</strong> output <strong>of</strong> syntax by failing to realize it.<br />

English ditransitives like give have two constructions available to <strong>the</strong>m, double<br />

object one in (43)a <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> prepositional one in (43)b, preferring one or <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

according to various factors (Krifka 2004, Levin <strong>and</strong> Rappaport Hovav 2005: 7.3,<br />

Bresnan <strong>and</strong> Nikitina forthc, Bruening 2010). English weak pronoun have a prosodic<br />

requirement that prevents <strong>the</strong>ir being separated from <strong>the</strong> verb (Williams<br />

1974: 69f., Zwicky 1986, Dowty 1995). In (43)a, this requirement renders <strong>the</strong><br />

double object construction unavailable, suspending all preferences (cf. Costa<br />

2003: 266). Verbs like bet or envy only allow <strong>the</strong> double object construction, (43)c<br />

(Emonds <strong>and</strong> Whitney 2005). This limitation comes into conflict with <strong>the</strong> prosodic<br />

requirements <strong>of</strong> weak pronouns in (43)c. The conflict cannot be 'repaired' in (43)d<br />

by <strong>the</strong> sudden emergence <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> prepositional construction, nor <strong>of</strong> ano<strong>the</strong>r o<strong>the</strong>rwise<br />

impossible structure such as <strong>the</strong>me + bare goal. The prosodic requirement<br />

renders <strong>the</strong> syntax <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se ditransitives unrealizable or ineffable, <strong>and</strong> do not affect<br />

syntax to license an o<strong>the</strong>rwise unavailable structure. It is particularly revealing because<br />

prosody does play a role in <strong>the</strong> above-mentioned preferences for using one<br />

or <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r construction – but only when both are available (Williams 1974:<br />

71f.). 10<br />

(43) a. We gave Kate <strong>the</strong> book/*it.<br />

b. We gave <strong>the</strong> book/it to Kate.<br />

c. We envied/bet Kate <strong>the</strong> book/*it.<br />

d. *We envied/bet <strong>the</strong> book/it (to) Kate.<br />

So it should be in <strong>the</strong> Y/T model for all <strong>the</strong> modules downstream from syntax.<br />

They may filter syntactic output by rendering some structures impossible, <strong>the</strong>y<br />

may impose <strong>the</strong>ir own preferences, but <strong>the</strong>y cannot affect syntax. Weaker models<br />

may add well-defined exceptions yet retain <strong>the</strong> generalization.<br />

For morphology, <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>sis corresponding to (42) is (44) (Zwicky 1996: 301,<br />

Miller, Pullum <strong>and</strong> Zwicky 1997: 68; Corbett <strong>and</strong> Baerman 2006; Halle <strong>and</strong> Marantz<br />

1993, Embick 2000).<br />

9 Many exceptions are too relative to particular assumptions to be generally persuasive: cf.<br />

Chomsky (2001: 23ff.) on <strong>the</strong> sensitivity <strong>of</strong> movement to <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> phonological content,<br />

Bobaljik (2002: 251-3) on that <strong>of</strong> scope to <strong>the</strong> linear position <strong>of</strong> copies, Tseng (2005) on that <strong>of</strong><br />

selection to phonological properties.<br />

10 English varieties with <strong>the</strong>me-goal double object structures allow envy it Kate (Haddican<br />

forthc).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!