26.11.2012 Views

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

frire 'fry' (e.g. 1PL *frions vs. rire 'laugh', 1PL rions), past for English forgo<br />

(*forwent, *forgoed), past participle for stride (*stridden, *strided), contracted<br />

form for am not (*amn't -- isn't, aren't), <strong>and</strong> subject-verb inversion in French for<br />

-je 'I' with most verbs (dois-tu/je 'must-you/I' but bois-tu/*je 'drink-you/*I'). These<br />

gaps may be wholly arbitrary synchronically, but syntactico-semantically arbitrary<br />

are also gaps motivated in o<strong>the</strong>r systems like <strong>the</strong> prosodic constraint in (43).<br />

Arbitrary gaps <strong>of</strong>ten occur where closely related systems have opaque cliticization<br />

<strong>and</strong> agreement. In (66)a, <strong>the</strong> 2PL.ERG→1PL.ABS combinations deletes <strong>the</strong><br />

object's phi-<strong>features</strong> in Bermeo Basque. The 1PL.ERG→2PL.ABS combination<br />

incurs a gap. Speakers resort to a paraphrase, like Bakosu guretsako lekorik ela?<br />

"Do you have room for us?" for *Erungosu gu? "Can you take us along?" (Egaña<br />

1984: 14). Lekeitio Basque lacks <strong>the</strong> agreement combination<br />

1PL.ERG→2PL.DAT, <strong>and</strong> speakers can only use nonagreeing allative <strong>and</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

postpositions to paraphrase <strong>the</strong> sense <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> agreeing dative, much as in (43)<br />

(Fernández 2001: 156). Wiltschko (2008: 309) compares <strong>the</strong> arbitrary agreement<br />

combinations where Chukchi uses <strong>the</strong> spurious antipassive (67) to Halkomelem<br />

gaps for (subject→object) 3→2 but not 3→1, <strong>and</strong> to Thompson River Salish gaps<br />

for 1PL→3 <strong>and</strong> 2SG→1PL but not elsewhere. There is one caveat. Opaque cliticization<br />

<strong>and</strong> agreement refer to phi-<strong>features</strong> ra<strong>the</strong>r than <strong>the</strong>ir exponents; it is less<br />

clear that arbitrary gaps ever do so. However, <strong>the</strong>re is evidence for it analogous to<br />

that mentioned for opaque agreement, including generalizations over phi-feature<br />

classes like interactions between 2 nd person <strong>and</strong> 1PL in Basque.<br />

The causes <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se gaps are sometimes understood <strong>and</strong> reinforce <strong>the</strong> impression<br />

<strong>of</strong> syntactico-semantic arbitrariness. Among <strong>the</strong>m are uncertainty in morpheme<br />

order or allomorph choice, anti-repetition constraints on morphological or<br />

phonological <strong>features</strong>, morphological garden paths, acquisition failures due to<br />

conflicting generalizations, <strong>and</strong> perhaps limits on feature complexity in morphemes:<br />

see Noyer (1992: 164-6), Heath (1991, 1998), Neeleman <strong>and</strong> van der<br />

Koot (2005), Albright (2006), Arregi <strong>and</strong> Nevins (2008), Rivero (2008), Wiltschko<br />

(2008), <strong>and</strong> Baerman, Corbett <strong>and</strong> Brown (2010).<br />

The gaps <strong>of</strong> interest here belong to <strong>the</strong> morphology or its use by <strong>the</strong> parserproducer.<br />

From this follow <strong>the</strong>ir properties. One is <strong>the</strong>ir definability within phrasestructurally<br />

'small' domain, to which morphology but not syntax is restricted by<br />

(69) (cf. Wiltschko op.cit.). Ano<strong>the</strong>r, central to modularity, is <strong>the</strong>ir syntacticosemantic<br />

inertness, brought out by Trommer (2002) <strong>and</strong> Embick <strong>and</strong> Marantz<br />

(2008). An expression that exists only in <strong>the</strong> context <strong>of</strong> a gap is its repair, whe<strong>the</strong>r<br />

licensed by <strong>the</strong> gap itself (Andrews 1990, Poser 1992, Williams 1997), or by a restatement<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> conditions that define <strong>the</strong> gap. The modular architecture <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

Y/T model claims that gaps defined at PF cannot have syntactico-semantic repairs.<br />

This is so. The missing past participle <strong>of</strong> English stride renders unavailable <strong>the</strong><br />

perfect (70)a, but does not license o<strong>the</strong>rwise unavailable syntactic structures (70)b<br />

or interpretations (70)c. The perfect <strong>of</strong> stride is ineffable.<br />

(70) a. *He has strid-?.<br />

49

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!