26.11.2012 Views

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

64<br />

morphology, it controls <strong>the</strong> agreement prefix n- for 1 st person, <strong>and</strong> determines <strong>the</strong><br />

–g suffix which indicates an inverse context where <strong>the</strong> O wins. In <strong>the</strong> syntax, this<br />

sentence satisfies <strong>the</strong> Ban on Inanimates, because <strong>the</strong> pivot is animate. In contrast,<br />

(90)d has <strong>the</strong> same same 3inanEA→1O combination, but in <strong>the</strong> conjunct order. In<br />

<strong>the</strong> conjunct, PH-interactions have been seen not to affect <strong>the</strong> morphology,<br />

indicating that <strong>the</strong> EA is always <strong>the</strong> pivot. For this reason (91)d remains ruled out<br />

by <strong>the</strong> Ban despite <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> animate O: <strong>the</strong> EA pivot is inanimate. The first<br />

verbal form gii-bsikwid is morphologically appropriate to <strong>the</strong> 3EA→1O conjunct<br />

with a grammatically animate EA mshiimin 'apple', but runs into <strong>the</strong> Ban on Inanimates.<br />

The second gii-bsikaagyaan is morphologically inappropriate because it<br />

treats mshiimin as if grammatically inanimate. No o<strong>the</strong>r verbal form is possible<br />

(Rhodes 1994: 145). The Ban illustrates <strong>the</strong> tight coupling <strong>of</strong> syntax <strong>and</strong> morphology<br />

in PH interactions. The 2 > 1 > 3 interaction is relevant to <strong>the</strong> syntactic Ban<br />

only if it affects morphology: in <strong>the</strong> independent order. 32<br />

The nature <strong>of</strong> constraints like <strong>the</strong> Ban on Inanimates is not fully understood.<br />

When a <strong>the</strong>matic role is constrained by animacy, selectional restrictions may be<br />

posited. However, EA-O PH-interactions go beyond selection, because in order to<br />

allow O to be inanimate or not, it is necessary to know what <strong>the</strong> <strong>features</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> EA<br />

are, <strong>and</strong> because EA <strong>and</strong> O may come from different clauses, as will be seen below.<br />

Instead, PH-interactions are usually formalized through movement-type dependencies<br />

whereby <strong>the</strong> winner/pivot moves to <strong>the</strong> highest (A-)position (section<br />

3.3). Even in English, tools are necessary to constrain A-movement by animacy,<br />

as Abney (1987: 176f., 205, 207f.) points out for (91). Possessive <strong>and</strong> unmarked<br />

subjects <strong>of</strong> gerunds may both involve raising, but <strong>the</strong> possessive prefers animates<br />

<strong>and</strong> excludes idiom chunks <strong>and</strong> expletives. On Abney's proposal (92)a, this is a selectional<br />

restriction on <strong>the</strong> possessive base-generated in [Spec, DP], which controls<br />

a PRO, which undergoes A-movement. A-movement is thus constrained by<br />

animacy indirectly. Ojibwa may lend itself to <strong>the</strong> same analysis, as in (92)b. 33<br />

(91) a. [John('s) seeming / being likely t to win] will only spur Bill on.<br />

b. I was surprised at [it(??'s) seeming that John might not win].<br />

c. I was irked at [advantage(??'s) being taken <strong>of</strong> John's situation].<br />

d. This justified [much(*'s) being made <strong>of</strong> Calvin's foresight.]<br />

(cf. Abney 1987: 177, 205, 208)<br />

32 The same Ban constraints 3↔3 contexts on <strong>the</strong> hierarchy 3.PROX→3.OBV. They are not illustrated<br />

here in detail, but <strong>the</strong>y provide supporting evidence for <strong>the</strong> syntactic status <strong>of</strong> obviation.<br />

The tree (inanimate) hit John (animate) requires by <strong>the</strong> Ban that John be <strong>the</strong> winner on <strong>the</strong> hierarchy<br />

<strong>and</strong> thus proximate, giving an inverse configuration where O outranks EA. By contrast,<br />

John hit <strong>the</strong> tree is fine only if John is proximate, a direct configuration. These direct/inverse distinctions<br />

are reflected in <strong>the</strong> morphology (Rhodes 1994: 433f.).<br />

33 At issue is whe<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> pivot can reconstruct, which it should not be able to do via PRO unlike<br />

via A-movement (Burzio 1986). Bruening (2001: 131) does demonstrate for <strong>the</strong> Algonquian language<br />

Passamaquoddy that <strong>the</strong> inverse <strong>of</strong> 3EA→3O combinations not only allows O > EA scope,<br />

but also reconstructed for EA > O scope, while <strong>the</strong> direct only allows EA > O.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!