26.11.2012 Views

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

66<br />

status, it affects syntax. As in <strong>the</strong> preceding diagnostic, <strong>the</strong>re is a tight coupling<br />

between morphology <strong>and</strong> syntax. Only in <strong>the</strong> independent order is <strong>the</strong> 2 > 1 > 3<br />

PH-interaction morphologically visible, <strong>and</strong> only <strong>the</strong>re does it matter for syntactic<br />

obviation. As for <strong>the</strong> mechanism <strong>of</strong> cross-clausal obviation, <strong>the</strong>re are various<br />

possibilities. One is that <strong>the</strong> winner on <strong>the</strong> PH-interaction moves to <strong>the</strong> highest Aposition,<br />

<strong>and</strong> from <strong>the</strong>re triggers obviation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> adjunct subject, ei<strong>the</strong>r<br />

syntactically, or through <strong>the</strong> information-structural prominence thus attained. 35<br />

The last diagnostic is cross-clausal agreement. Certain matrix verbs like<br />

'think' agree with a designated argument α in <strong>the</strong>ir clausal complement as if it were<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir own O (Dahlstrom 1986, Branigan <strong>and</strong> MacKenzie 2001, Bruening 2001,<br />

Lochbihler 2008 in Algonquian, <strong>and</strong> generally Polinsky 2003). The situations <strong>of</strong><br />

interest are those where α can interact with ano<strong>the</strong>r argument on <strong>the</strong> 2 > 1 > 3 hierarchy.<br />

The embedded clauses are conjunct, so <strong>the</strong> 2 > 1 > 3 PH interaction does<br />

not occur, <strong>and</strong> α is always <strong>the</strong> EA (Rhodes 1994: 438f.). However, if <strong>the</strong> matrix<br />

clause is in <strong>the</strong> independent order, α does interact with its EA. Bruening (2001)<br />

shows that in <strong>the</strong> Algonquian language Passamaquoddy, this interaction has syntactic<br />

consequences. If α is lower-ranked than <strong>the</strong> matrix EA according 2 > 1 > 3<br />

hierarchy (matrix direct context), α must link to a movement gap within <strong>the</strong> embedded<br />

clause, by a path free <strong>of</strong> isl<strong>and</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> if it appears overtly, it is at <strong>the</strong> edge<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> embedded clause. Thus in (94), <strong>the</strong> EA wins <strong>the</strong> 1EA→3O EA-α interaction,<br />

as seen by <strong>the</strong> prefix 1 st person n- on 'know', <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>refore <strong>the</strong> 3 rd person α 'who'<br />

cannot come from an adjunct isl<strong>and</strong> within <strong>the</strong> embedded clause. In (95) on <strong>the</strong><br />

o<strong>the</strong>r h<strong>and</strong>, α outranks <strong>the</strong> matrix EA in <strong>the</strong> 3EA→1O <strong>and</strong> 1EA→2O combinations,<br />

controlling <strong>the</strong> prefixes 1 st person n- <strong>and</strong> 2 nd person k- on 'know'. Then α links to a<br />

resumptive pronoun in <strong>the</strong> embedded clause, in disregard <strong>of</strong> isl<strong>and</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> may appear<br />

overtly within <strong>the</strong> matrix clause itself. 36<br />

(94) a. N-kosiciy-a [wen elomi-ya-t [mesq Mali mace-ntu-hk]].<br />

1-know.TA-DIR who IC.away-go-3.CJ not.yet M. start-sing-3.CJ.NEG<br />

IEA know whoα tα left before Mary started singing.<br />

b. *N-kosiciy-a [wen elomi-ya-t Mihku [mesq t mace-ntu-hk]].<br />

1-know.TA-DIR who IC.away-go-3.CJ M. not.yet start-sing-3.CJ.NEG<br />

IEA know whoα Mihku left before t started singing.<br />

(Bruening 2001: 268, matrix pivot EA in bold)<br />

(95) a. N-kosiciy-oq al nikuwoss [eli psi=te wen<br />

1-know.TA-INV uncertain 1.mo<strong>the</strong>r C all=EMPH someone<br />

35 See fur<strong>the</strong>r Bruening (2001), Mühlbauer (2008) on obviation. Superficially, <strong>the</strong> alternation in<br />

<strong>the</strong> obviation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> adjunct subject is reminiscent <strong>of</strong> that between anaphor types in sentences <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> type Lynni found Katek tired when xi/k came home, in languages like Spanish, Finnish, or<br />

German. If xi picks up <strong>the</strong> subject, it tends to be pro ra<strong>the</strong>r than a pronoun, or a pronoun ra<strong>the</strong>r<br />

than a demonstrative, <strong>and</strong> inversely if xk picks up <strong>the</strong> object (Turan 1996, Arnold 1998, Kaiser<br />

2003).<br />

36 The 3.PROX > 3.OBV PH-interaction, which applies in <strong>the</strong> conjunct as well as independent<br />

order, matters both for determining α in <strong>the</strong> lower clause <strong>and</strong> for Bruening's generalization.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!