26.11.2012 Views

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

90<br />

Dative <strong>and</strong> locative à preposition or case-marker have <strong>the</strong> same morphology<br />

(Miller 1992: 4.5.5.4, 248 note 14, Roberge <strong>and</strong> Troberg 2007). However, <strong>the</strong> two<br />

correspond to morphologically distinct clitic series <strong>and</strong> have different syntax.<br />

These differences are set out in <strong>the</strong> following list under several headings. They<br />

will be important throughout this chapter. Their usual <strong>the</strong>oretical interpretation is<br />

through a DP-PP distinction originating with Kayne (1975: 2.II-III). It is preliminarily<br />

set out in (125), <strong>and</strong> fur<strong>the</strong>r articulated in section 4.7. Locative arguments<br />

are full PPs <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir clitics are pro-PPs, (125)c. Accusatives <strong>and</strong> accusative clitics<br />

are (pro-)DPs, (125)a. Datives are in between, especially dative clitics. Their PDAT<br />

à is 'weaker' than <strong>the</strong> full preposition PLOC à, <strong>and</strong> in clitics perhaps need not project<br />

over D, (125)c. They are defective PPs. (Genitives pattern with locatives.)<br />

(125) a. ACC: D b. DAT: [D/P? PDAT D] c. LOC: [P PLOC D] (clitic)<br />

DP [PP? PDAT DP] [PP PLOC DP] (nonclitic)<br />

– Agreement <strong>and</strong> Case: Only accusatives participate in <strong>the</strong> nominativeaccusative<br />

Case alternation or, as clitics, control participle agreement. 58<br />

A temporary resident must obey <strong>the</strong>m.Di / (?)LOCi, to <strong>the</strong>se legal measuresi.<br />

(Couquaux 1975: 35, Blanche-Benveniste 1978: 6)<br />

(ii) Le départementi de breton a besoin d'une secrétaire. Il ne nous reste que Claire/toi. On<br />

va {la/*te lui attribuer} / {t'/*l'attribuer à lui} pour un an.<br />

The department(M)i <strong>of</strong> Breton needs a secretary. We have only Claire/you left. We<br />

will attribute {her.A/*you.A it.Di} / {you.A/*her.A to him.Di} for one year.<br />

Genuine locations do not permit <strong>the</strong> locative clitic to refer to humans <strong>and</strong> need à + strong pronoun,<br />

e.g. venir 'come to', amèner 'bring (someone) to'. A class <strong>of</strong> verbs selects locative arguments<br />

with a less clearly locative meaning <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir clitics may <strong>the</strong>n refer to humans, with some<br />

variation: e.g. penser 'think about', réflechir 'reflect on', songer 'dream about', habituer 'habituate<br />

to', interesser 'interest in', s'attacher 'attach oneself to', faire attention 'pay attention to', faire<br />

peur 'frighten' (Kayne 1975: 106-9, Gaatone 1984, Herslund 1988: 2.3, 3.2.1, Jones 1996: 262;<br />

CNRTL s.v. y 2 II.B.2.a remarque 1, Grevisse <strong>and</strong> Goosse 2008: §678.2°, Pinchon 1972: 2.III,<br />

Nyrop 1925: §221). One set <strong>of</strong> grammars permits <strong>the</strong> y <strong>of</strong> such verbs to refer to humans <strong>and</strong> even<br />

1 st /2 nd person provided <strong>the</strong>y are not <strong>the</strong> subject <strong>of</strong> consciousness (Ruwet 1990: 80f., 1991, Pica<br />

1994; cf. Blanche-Benveniste 1978, Lamiroy 1990, 1991, Zribi-Hertz 2000). Ano<strong>the</strong>r freely<br />

permits human reference (Couquaux 1975, Lambrecht 1981: 35). See Rezac (2010c) for a comparison<br />

<strong>and</strong> relationship to <strong>the</strong> PCC repair. Matters are similar for <strong>the</strong> genitive clitic en, save that<br />

it admits human reference freely as partitive.<br />

58 On participle agreement, see Kayne (1989), D'Aless<strong>and</strong>ro <strong>and</strong> Roberts (2008). Reflexive clitic<br />

datives exceptionally agree for some speakers, (i): Grevisse <strong>and</strong> Goosse (2008: §953), Medová<br />

(2009: 5.3, 6.3.2), vs. Burzio (1981: 482, 446), Kayne (2000: 24 note 21); cf. for Italian, Belletti<br />

(2005: 2.2), D'Aless<strong>and</strong>ro <strong>and</strong> Roberts (2008: 484 note 8). Reflexive clitics are syncretic for dative<br />

<strong>and</strong> accusative, although differentiated by linked elements such 'each o<strong>the</strong>r' in (i), but nonreflexive<br />

1 st /2 nd person clitics are also syncretic yet agree only if accusative. The participle agreement<br />

<strong>of</strong> reflexive accusative <strong>and</strong> dative reflexive clitics has been related to <strong>the</strong> fact that through<br />

reflexivity <strong>the</strong>ir phi-<strong>features</strong> are <strong>the</strong> same as those <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> subject, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> perfect auxiliary involved<br />

in reflexives is BE ra<strong>the</strong>r than HAVE, so it is really <strong>the</strong> subject <strong>of</strong> BE that controls parti-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!