26.11.2012 Views

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

Phi-features and the Modular Architecture of - UMR 7023 - CNRS

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

see her, PRO to see her, your/you seeing her. These are syntactic properties <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

designated subject position [Spec, TP/FinP]. Mapudugun is special because what<br />

occupies this position is determined by <strong>the</strong> EA-O PH-interaction (cf. Baker 2003).<br />

For 1↔2 combinations, both <strong>the</strong> morphology <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> syntactic facts are unclear.<br />

Thus as in Algonquian, <strong>the</strong> Mapudungun evidence establishes that <strong>the</strong> 1/2 > 3 portion<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> PH hierarchy has a syntactic correlate. 39<br />

(96) a. mule-y mün allkü-tu-ñma-ya-fi-el ñi dungu (direct)<br />

have-3SG your(PL) listen-?-SRC-FUT-OBJ-VN his word<br />

You must listen to his word.<br />

b. fey muna kutran-ka-w-üy mi trem-üm-a-t-ew (inverse)<br />

she very illness-FAC-REFL-3SG your(SG) grow-CAUS-FUT-INV-<br />

VNOM-3(.O)<br />

She made a lot <strong>of</strong> sacrifices in order to raise you.<br />

(Arnold 1994: 38, 1997 ex. 46, 47) 40<br />

Algonquian <strong>and</strong> Mapudungun make a robust case for syntactico-semantic correlates<br />

to PH-interactions. The correlates closely track <strong>the</strong> morphological manifestation<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> PH-interaction, strikingly so in <strong>the</strong> Algonquian independent-conjunct<br />

contrast. The morphological <strong>and</strong> syntactic PH-interactions thus clearly reflect <strong>the</strong><br />

same mechanism.<br />

The languages reviewed here are not alone in having potential syntactic correlates<br />

<strong>of</strong> PH-interactions, but ra<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong> ones where <strong>the</strong> evidence is clearer <strong>and</strong><br />

bears specifically on phi-<strong>features</strong> ra<strong>the</strong>r than on notions like obviation. Agreement<br />

<strong>and</strong> case-marking have been excluded as evidence for <strong>the</strong> syntactic status <strong>of</strong> PHinteractions,<br />

since <strong>the</strong>ir syntactic or morphological character is precisely <strong>the</strong> issue,<br />

in <strong>the</strong> face <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ories that would put <strong>the</strong>m into an enriched morphology, one that<br />

can span <strong>the</strong> phrase-structural distances seen in cross-clausal agreement (Marantz<br />

2000, Halle <strong>and</strong> Marantz 1993, Bobaljik 2008). Also excluded for <strong>the</strong> same reason<br />

have been phenomena where PH-interaction affects only <strong>the</strong> presence, order, or<br />

shape <strong>of</strong> affixes or clitics. In Cavineña for instance, <strong>the</strong> 1 > 2 > 3 hierarchy determines<br />

<strong>the</strong> order <strong>of</strong> second position enclitics for <strong>the</strong> subject <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> direct object<br />

(Guillaume 2006), while in French, <strong>the</strong> 1/2 > 3 hierarchy determines <strong>the</strong> order <strong>of</strong><br />

object proclitics to <strong>the</strong> verb (Laenzlinger 1993). 41 Similar phenomena appear to be<br />

39 In 1↔2 scenarios, Arnold (1994: 38, 1997) observes that <strong>the</strong> EL-forms choose 2 nd person in<br />

1EA→2O <strong>and</strong> 2EA→1O, which Zuñiga (2002: 235) qualifies. O<strong>the</strong>r syntactic correlates <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> EA-<br />

O interaction in Mapudungun bear on or have only been tested for 3.PROX > 3.OBV. For instance,<br />

wh-movement applies to <strong>the</strong> O <strong>of</strong> morphologically direct <strong>and</strong> to <strong>the</strong> EA <strong>of</strong> morphologically<br />

inverse clauses, recalling <strong>the</strong> cross-linguistically common ban on ergative extraction <strong>and</strong> its<br />

repair in some Mayan languages fix by demotion to absolutive (Berinstein 1985, 1990, Davies<br />

<strong>and</strong> Sam Colop 1990, Hale 2001, Aissen 1999, Béjar <strong>and</strong> Rezac 2007: Appendix).<br />

40 The inverse e is missing by assimilation to a (Arnold 1994: 40 note 10, 1997 note 14). The<br />

gloss <strong>of</strong> mün has been changed from you to your on <strong>the</strong> basis <strong>of</strong> Arnold's discussion.<br />

41 I am grateful to A. Guillaume for discussion <strong>of</strong> Cavineña <strong>and</strong> providing me with his work.<br />

69

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!