07.02.2015 Views

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

nine steps of their technique “[t]he interrogator [...] will be meeting all of the<br />

guidelines established by the courts in conducting proper interrogations to develop<br />

admissible confessions from guilty suspects.” 172<br />

Decision-Making Model<br />

Hilgendorf and Irving have suggested an alternative concept of interrogations<br />

and confessions. 173 Their model provides a framework for analyzing “the<br />

circumstances in which any particular confession was made in terms of the<br />

decision-making task of the suspect, the information with which he is provided,<br />

the social pressures which are brought to bear on him, and the physical character<br />

of the interrogation.” 174 It conceptualizes interrogation as a complicated and<br />

demanding decision-making process. 175 The subject of an interrogation must make<br />

many choices, some of which include whether to speak or remain silent; whether<br />

to make self-incriminating admissions or a confession; whether to tell the truth,<br />

part of the truth, or lie; how to answer the questions asked by the interrogator; and<br />

what attitude to adopt toward the police. 176<br />

Hilgendorf and Irving’s model predicts that subjects will seek to make the<br />

best possible choice among the courses of action available by choosing “that<br />

course for which the product of (1) the probability of occurrence and (2) the<br />

value to him (or utility) of the consequences, is largest.” 177 The subject’s action<br />

will have consequences for him or her: criminal charges may be filed; he or she<br />

may be detained; the police may check the information provided for accuracy<br />

and truthfulness. 178 Thus, he or she will attempt to evaluate the probabilities of<br />

each consequence’s occurrence, and his or her decision about how to act will be<br />

a “result of some balancing of the likelihood of various consequences in relation<br />

to their utilities for him [or her].” 179 Consequently, an interrogation subject’s<br />

decisions are determined by:<br />

• Perceptions of the available courses of action.<br />

• Perceptions concerning the probabilities of the likely occurrence of<br />

various consequences attached to these courses of action.<br />

• The utility values or gains attached to these courses of action.<br />

Hilgendorf and Irving make clear that the subject’s decision making is<br />

governed not by the objective probabilities that given consequences may occur,<br />

but by the subjective probabilities of their occurrence. 180 In other words, decisions<br />

172<br />

Id.<br />

173<br />

E.L. Hilgendorf and B. Irving, “A Decision-Making Model of Confessions,” in Psychology in<br />

Legal Contexts: Applications and Limitations, M.A. Lloyd-Bostock, ed. (London, UK: Macmillan,<br />

1981), 67-84.<br />

174<br />

Id., p. 81.<br />

175<br />

Id., p. 69.<br />

176<br />

Id.<br />

177<br />

Id.<br />

178<br />

Id.<br />

179<br />

Id., p. 70.<br />

180<br />

Id., p. 71.<br />

150

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!