07.02.2015 Views

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

that the other party will refuse to act. In the game of chicken, for example, the<br />

driver who supposedly threw the steering wheel out of the window might have<br />

thrown a replica — and kept the real wheel out of view of the other driver.<br />

Crafting a “Yesable Proposition”<br />

Even when a negotiator has developed a good proposal that creates value and<br />

distributes that value effectively, parties can fail to reach agreement because of<br />

the manner in which the proposal is framed.<br />

The Power of Framing<br />

Framing can have a subtle but powerful impact on how the other side<br />

perceives a proposal. For example, studies have found that negotiators instructed<br />

to “minimize losses” rather than “maximize gains” were less likely to make<br />

concessions, reach agreement, and view the resulting agreement as fair (Bazerman,<br />

Magliozzi, and Neale, 1985; Neale and Northcraft, 1986; Neale, Huber, and<br />

Northcraft, 1987).<br />

The context in which a decision is framed can also affect how it is perceived.<br />

One study found that participants would be willing to walk two blocks to save<br />

$30 on a $70 watch, but not willing to walk that same distance to save $30 on an<br />

$800 camera (Russo and Schoemaker 1989). Thirty dollars seems like a great deal<br />

of money when compared to $70, but like a drop in the bucket when compared<br />

to $800.<br />

The identity of the person making the offer also influences its reception. A<br />

study by Bazerman and Neale found that participants would be willing to pay<br />

more for a bottle of beer they were told came from a fancy resort than for exactly<br />

the same bottle supposedly from a run-down grocery store. Participants assumed<br />

that the grocery store beer “is an obvious rip-off” (1992). Thus, it is important to<br />

consider both who presents the offer and how it is presented.<br />

Framing a “Yesable” Proposition<br />

Ultimately, the question to ask is: What proposal would give the other side<br />

an option they might accept The choice would have to address their interests<br />

sufficiently, be realistic, and be operational. By having a good sense of the other<br />

party’s interests and BATNA, a negotiator can craft such an offer, which is called<br />

a “yesable proposition” (Fisher, Kopelman, and Schneider, 1994): it requires only<br />

a “yes” in response.<br />

Rather than confronting the other party with a problem, a yesable proposition<br />

gives them an appealing offer. Consider a simple example. President Lyndon<br />

Johnson instructed his staff to attach a proposal, and a set of boxes for him to<br />

check “yes,” “no,” or “see me,” to any memo that crossed his desk. Johnson<br />

understood the idea behind a yesable proposition: he required his staff to bring<br />

him not only a problem, but also a suggestion for what could be done.<br />

Two tools are useful in developing a yesable proposition: the Currently<br />

Perceived Choice chart and the Target Future Choice chart.<br />

276

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!