07.02.2015 Views

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

of feet when there is a direct line of sight. EMMA is an eye tracking system that<br />

follows the pattern of a subject’s visual attention to a scene, specifically how the<br />

subject’s eye scans a familiar object versus an unfamiliar object. The DoDPI has<br />

published no review studies to date. These methods may hold promise, but are not<br />

currently ready for deployment.<br />

Polygraph<br />

Subtle bodily changes such as heart rate, blood pressure, and skin resistance<br />

are amplified and recorded onto a multichannel writing instrument known as the<br />

polygraph. The polygraph is typically used in conjunction with one or more of a set<br />

of related tests: the Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT), also known as the Concealed<br />

<strong>Information</strong> Test (CIT) (Ben-Shakhar and Elaad, 2002); the Comparison Question<br />

Test or Control Question Test (CQT) (Raskin and Honts, 2002); or the Relevant/<br />

Irrelevant Test (RIT) (Iacono, 2000). It should be noted that although the CQT<br />

is the most widely used test in North America, its scientific validity has been<br />

criticized (Gronau et al., 2005; Ben-Shakhar et al., 2002). In contrast, the GKT/<br />

CIT, widely used only in Japan, has been supported as an objective test (Gronau<br />

et al., 2005; Ben-Shakhar et al., 2002).<br />

The typical method of scoring the physiological records is for the polygrapher<br />

to look globally at the charts for a “lie response”: an assumed specific response<br />

pattern uniquely associated with lying. Another, more objective, method of scoring<br />

the charts is to measure the relative magnitude of the responses (Backster, 1962);<br />

that is, when the difference between the relevant and control question response<br />

levels reaches a certain quantitative point, the decision is made to classify the<br />

examinee as deceptive.<br />

The polygraph is the most widely employed technical means for detecting<br />

deception. It is also perhaps the most controversial. Almost since its introduction,<br />

polygraphy and its direct technological ancestors have been the subjects of legal<br />

proceedings and Supreme Court decisions, including the Frye case (Frye v. United<br />

States, 54 App. D.C. 46, 293 F. 1013 [1923, D.C. Cir.], in which the testimony of<br />

polygraph pioneer William Marsten was excluded) and the more recent Scheffer<br />

case (United States v. Scheffer, 523 U.S. 303 [1998]). Ironically, the federal<br />

government relies extensively on the polygraph for forensic investigations and<br />

personnel security, yet the results of polygraph tests are generally inadmissible in<br />

federal courts (Greeley, 2004).<br />

Potential<br />

The accuracy of the polygraph is a matter of controversy. Some researchers<br />

believe that the current system with the CQT is no better than chance (c.f. Ben-<br />

Shakhar, 1991; Furedy, 1996; Saxe, 1991). Other researchers estimate the accuracy<br />

at 75% to 80% (i.e., one error, on average, in four to five trials): Elaad and others<br />

(1992) with the GKT/CIT, MacLaren (2001) with the GKT/CIT, and Patrick and<br />

Iacono (1991) with the CQT. Supreme Court Justice John P. Stevens found “a host<br />

of studies that place the reliability of polygraph tests at 85 to 90 percent” (United<br />

States v. Scheffer, 523 U.S. 303, 333, quoted in Greeley, 2004, p. 129).<br />

65

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!