07.02.2015 Views

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

Educing Information: Interrogation - National Intelligence University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

After drilling a small hole in the temple on each side of the<br />

skull, the surgeon then inserts a dull knife into the brain,<br />

makes a fan-shaped incision through the prefrontal lobe, then<br />

downward a few minutes later. He then repeats the incision on<br />

the other side of the brain.... The patient is given only local<br />

anesthetic at the temples — the brain itself is insensitive—and<br />

the doctors encourage him to talk, sing, recite poems or prayers.<br />

When his replies to questions show that his mind is thoroughly<br />

disoriented, the doctors know that they have cut deep enough<br />

into his brain. 690<br />

How could this procedure receive rave reviews Apparently, the flaw in<br />

these early studies was simply that physicians who provided these treatments also<br />

rated the results. We know of no evidence to suggest that these physicians were<br />

intentionally biased or deceitful; or that they had anything other than the best<br />

interest of their patients at heart.<br />

For a more contemporary example, consider the presumed ability of law<br />

enforcement interrogators to evaluate their subjects. Professional interrogators<br />

view nonverbal cues as important for detecting deception. Such cues include level<br />

of eye contact; movement of legs, feet, head and trunk; shifting body positions;<br />

“covering gestures” such as a hand over the mouth while talking; ear tugging; etc.<br />

All of these have been tested; none is substantiated as an indicator of deception. 691<br />

A similar result occurs when interrogators are asked to determine (on the basis<br />

of case summaries and interrogation videotapes) whether a confession is true or<br />

false. Most studies show that trained and untrained evaluators (e.g., police officers<br />

and college students) are equally poor at distinguishing between the confessions<br />

of guilty and innocent study subjects, even when viewing videotaped interviews<br />

from law enforcement situations. Often, however, law enforcement personnel<br />

have more confidence in their abilities than untrained subjects — even though<br />

their detection capabilities are no better. 692<br />

Associated with such examples is an extensive scientific literature on human<br />

judgment. We will not review this literature here, but simply note that the examples<br />

noted above should not be considered atypical. 693 Natural human judgment biases,<br />

such as the Law of Small Numbers (the tendency to jump to conclusions on the<br />

basis of too little data) and the Confi rmation bias (the tendency to underweight or<br />

ignore evidence inconsistent with current beliefs), are very strong. These biases<br />

690<br />

This description of the history of lobotomy was drawn from R. Dawes, House of Cards:<br />

Psychology and Psychotherapy Built on Myth (New York, The Free Press, 1994).<br />

691<br />

Research on detection deception is summarized in G. Hazlett, “Detection of Deception Research<br />

Review,” prepared under the auspices of this ISB study.<br />

692<br />

See S. M. Kassin, Meissner, and R. J. Norwick, ”I’d Know a False Confession If I Saw One”:<br />

A Comparative Study of College Students and Police Investigators. Law and Human Behavior, 29<br />

(2005), 211-227.<br />

693<br />

For review of this research see R. Hastie, and R. Dawes, Rational Choice in an Uncertain<br />

World: The Psychology of Judgment and Decision Making (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications,<br />

2001).<br />

304

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!