06.09.2021 Views

Australian Politics and Policy - Senior, 2019a

Australian Politics and Policy - Senior, 2019a

Australian Politics and Policy - Senior, 2019a

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Voter behaviour<br />

How citizens learn<br />

Political <strong>and</strong> social psychology provide substantial critiques on citizens’ capacities<br />

to perform their democratic duties, helping us reconcile voters’ limitations with the<br />

idea that democracies work reasonably well.<br />

Voters do not necessarily need detailed knowledge about politics <strong>and</strong> policy<br />

to fulfil their democratic duty. They can be thought of as ‘cognitive misers’, who<br />

minimise the effort involved in making potentially complex or difficult decisions<br />

using shortcuts, learning only as much as they need to <strong>and</strong> receiving <strong>and</strong><br />

interpreting signals from elected officials, opinion leaders <strong>and</strong> other sources.<br />

One way voters make political choices (such as choosing who to vote for) without<br />

a substantial investment in information gathering is through the use of heuristics,<br />

or cognitive shortcuts. 26 These are also used when making non-political decisions.<br />

Individuals are using a heuristic, for instance, when they fix their beliefs more<br />

heavily on the first piece of information they receive (the ‘anchor’) when making<br />

a decision. This is known as the anchoring heuristic. An example is the first price<br />

mentioned during a negotiation. If a salesperson offers a very high price to start<br />

negotiations, this becomes a psychological anchor for the buyer, meaning the<br />

counter-offer<strong>and</strong>finalpricearemorelikelytobehigherthanotherwise.<br />

The representativeness heuristic is another cognitive shortcut. This involves<br />

comparing a problem or decision to the most representative mental prototype.<br />

When a voter is trying to decide if a politician is trustworthy, they might compare<br />

that politician’s characteristics to other people they have known in the past. If<br />

the politician shares traits with a kind gr<strong>and</strong>father or harsh teacher, they might<br />

beassumedtobegentle<strong>and</strong>trustworthyorcritical<strong>and</strong>mean.Thisresultsin<br />

classifications that may or may not be correct, but saves on the effort of seeking<br />

additional information for critical analysis.<br />

Party identification can also be thought of as a form of heuristic that guides<br />

voter behaviour. 27 This helps to make politics less cognitively dem<strong>and</strong>ing for voters.<br />

Once citizens decide which party generally represents them, this single piece of<br />

information can act as a shortcut guiding how they view issues <strong>and</strong> events. A policy<br />

championed by the party this voter identifies with is more likely to meet with favour<br />

than the same policy advocated by a different party. For instance, if the Liberal<br />

Party promotes a new policy, voters who identify as Liberal supporters may be more<br />

inclined to believe this is a good idea than if the Labor Party had proposed the same<br />

policy.<br />

Party identification can also guide how we view events. Bartels showed that<br />

voters’ statements about objective facts, such as whether unemployment increased<br />

or decreased, were heavily influenced by party identification. 28 Under Republican<br />

26 Kahneman, Slovic <strong>and</strong> Tversky 1982.<br />

27 Campbell et al. 1960.<br />

28 Bartels 2002.<br />

477

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!