04.06.2014 Views

Download this publication - PULP

Download this publication - PULP

Download this publication - PULP

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

352 Chapter 15<br />

data is poured into algorithms that spew out probabilities generally borne<br />

out in reality. Remember probabilities are statistical averages: However,<br />

if an algorithm supported by the evidence and the probabilities tells you a<br />

left-handed batter virtually never hits to the left and opposite side of the<br />

field, then you would do well to shift your fielders predominantly to the<br />

right side. (The ‘shift’ works – though not all the time.) War games offer a<br />

similar opportunity to test a battlefield strategy before a war has begun – to<br />

see how it will play out. (Politicians – unlike military officers – often ignore<br />

the war game’s tested exit strategy, given that it was not the reason for<br />

military action in the first place and (generally) has no effect on their<br />

subsequent attempts at re-election.) Baseball coaches who similarly ignore<br />

algorithms and continually forget to form a viable exit strategy are invariably<br />

fired.<br />

I have, in passing, mentioned the huge amount of film that coaches and<br />

players watch in order to form an accurate picture of what they are about to<br />

confront. To suggest that the manner in which information is organised and<br />

hypothesised about – as Fish does – is not ‘a set of theories’ suggests a vexed<br />

philosopher and not an avid baseball fan. Fish is certainly correct in<br />

suggesting that Dworkin is rather clueless about baseball. No batter goes up to<br />

the plate with a theory of everything, let alone a theory before each pitch.<br />

However, batters have spent inordinate amounts of time watching pitchers<br />

(pitches are even charted as the game occurs in real time to tell future batters<br />

what to expect), and they go up to the plate with pre-dispositions based upon<br />

their own strengths and the perceived weaknesses of the pitchers on the other<br />

team. A batter might think: ‘He can’t get his curveball in for a strike, I will<br />

wait on his fastball’. Or a team might think, ‘his split-finger fast ball’ (a<br />

slightly slower pitch with lots of movement) is ‘falling off the table tonight’.<br />

(That’s a good thing for the pitcher because it completely fools the batter. The<br />

batter ‘sees’ a straight hard pitch aimed around his belt only to have it arrive<br />

at the plate more slowly and dive down around his ankles.) The batter has a<br />

response, since the split-finger change-up is often not a strike (though it may<br />

initially look like it will be one). The batter (and his teammates and coaches)<br />

may think – theorise – that since <strong>this</strong> effective pitch is not going to be a strike,<br />

‘I’ll wait on a fastball’. (Of course, that is extremely hard to do. The pitch is<br />

designed to look like a fastball as the pitcher releases it, and as we have shall<br />

see in a moment, no time exists in which to make conscious adaptations.)<br />

These theories – based on past and immediate experience – the batter takes<br />

with him to the plate.<br />

Fish overplayed his hand.<br />

Fish would have been well-served by stopping his argument after<br />

catching Dworkin out in the beginning. But, again, he makes a gamelosing<br />

error by continuing his attack in <strong>this</strong> game of GOTCHA. Ty Cobb<br />

did not understand that the time between the release of the ball by the pitcher<br />

and its arrival at home plate (.45 seconds) is faster than the conscious

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!