04.06.2014 Views

Download this publication - PULP

Download this publication - PULP

Download this publication - PULP

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

The relationship between theory and practice across forms of life 353<br />

awareness of the pitch (.6 seconds). 27 No batter can employ a grand theory<br />

before every pitch. What would such a theory tell you since the action<br />

would outpace – in real time – the conscious awareness of the pitch and<br />

the swing? Yes: That’s right – the hitter has initiated his swing before he is<br />

consciously aware of the fact that he has swung. 28 [Recall Cobb’s error.]<br />

However, Fish has made a grand mistake about consciousness and<br />

theorising. Baseball players, after 10 000 hours of playing and more, have<br />

well-established pre-dispositional states that allow them to react more<br />

quickly to novel situations than they are consciously aware. 29 They have<br />

built-in theories that allow for extremely subtle adjustments to the pitch<br />

headed in their direction. 30<br />

However, it does not follow from the batter’s habitual – as opposed to<br />

conscious – response that ‘theory’ plays no role in baseball. From pitch<br />

counting (how tired is a pitcher likely to be after throwing 90 pitches, and<br />

thus in less command of his repertoire), from watching his location (is he in<br />

control or command of his pitches from the very beginning) to the batter’s<br />

own self-awareness, a batter may walk about to the plate with a ‘feel’ –<br />

an extremely well-informed feel – for what must be done. Fish is<br />

absolutely, categorically wrong about the (lack of) existence of theory in<br />

baseball. It exists – in an extremely refined fashion that proves of great use<br />

to all of its practitioners.<br />

But of course there is no time for conscious reflection as one initiates a<br />

swing. We practise. We may reflect between pitches. We may take lessons<br />

from one day and apply them to the next. But we still have to play – and we<br />

play at our best after years of practice and ‘theorising’ about what works and<br />

what does not. So pace Fish: We spend a career practising and theorising about<br />

the game – the two work hand in glove to enable us to do what comes<br />

27 The work of empirical psychologists Benjamin Libet and W Grey Walters has<br />

provided a well-established framework for understanding delayed conscious awareness<br />

of ‘unconsciously’ initiated action. See, eg, B Libet ‘The experimental evidence for<br />

subjective referral of evidence backward in time: Reply to PS Churchland’ (1981) 48<br />

Philosophy of Science 182; B Libet et al ‘Time of conscious intention to act in relation to<br />

the onset of other cerebral activities (readiness potential): The unconscious initiation of<br />

a freely voluntary act’ (1983) 106 Brain 623; Walters’ Presentation to the Osler Society<br />

(1963) as reported in D Dennett Consciousness explained (1991) 167 - 171. Some readers<br />

might want to know how would one explain the fact that while a baseball travels the 60<br />

feet and 6 inches from pitcher to hitter in 0.45 seconds, Libet’s experiments seem to reflect<br />

a much more generous period of 0.8 seconds between readiness potential and action. The<br />

explanation is agent- priming. Constant habituation enables actors to shorten<br />

dramatically the period between non-conscious intention and action.<br />

28 See, eg, B Libet Mind time: The temporal factor in consciousness (2004).<br />

29<br />

See D Dennett Consciousness explained (1991); D Dennett Elbow room: The varieties of<br />

freedom worth wanting (1984); Dennett Freedom evolves (2003).<br />

30 See DJ Baars ‘How does a serial, integrated and very limited stream of consciousness<br />

emerge from a nervous system that is mostly unconscious, distributed, parallel and of<br />

enormous capacity’ in GR Bock & J Marsh (eds) Experimental and theoretical studies of<br />

consciousness (1993) 282. See also S Dehaene & L Naccache ‘Towards a cognitive<br />

neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework’ in<br />

S Dehaene (ed) Cognitive neuroscience of consciousness (2001) 1 9 - 12.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!