06.04.2013 Views

Johnson 2004 - CDLI - UCLA

Johnson 2004 - CDLI - UCLA

Johnson 2004 - CDLI - UCLA

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

inanimate noun in an ablative/instrumental case. Garrett describes the morphosyntactic<br />

environment as well as its justification as a locus of change as follows:<br />

According to this scheme, instrumental NPs in null-subject transitive clauses . . .<br />

are reanalyzed diachronically as subjects. Such a reanalysis is permitted because<br />

of the functional overlap between instrument and agent in clauses with transitive<br />

predicates: cf. e.g. John opened the door with the key and the key opened the<br />

door, or John extinguished the fire with water and water extinguished the fire. A<br />

similar reanalysis cannot occur in clauses with intransitive predicates because<br />

thematic instruments are rare or absent altogether in the subject position of<br />

intransitive clauses. (Garrett 1990, 265)<br />

I would argue that that [+locus] component of the ablative/instrumental case that<br />

Garrett’s model calls for is coded by the old locative postposition, *-e, whereas the<br />

directionality of the ablative/instrumental case is coded by the low source applicative,<br />

*bi-.<br />

Given these constraints, it may be useful at this point to imagine two templates for<br />

such an environment that differ only in terms of the (in)alienability of the bare noun.<br />

(75) NOUN animate-e (GOAL) NOUN inanimate-e (SOURCE) NOUN inalienable bi-√<br />

(76) NOUN animate-e (GOAL) NOUN inanimate-e (SOURCE) NOUN alienable bi-√<br />

In the old system, in which both perceived object (SOURCE) and perceiver (GOAL)<br />

appeared in the same old locative case, *-e, the transfer of possession model was fairly<br />

325

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!