06.04.2013 Views

Johnson 2004 - CDLI - UCLA

Johnson 2004 - CDLI - UCLA

Johnson 2004 - CDLI - UCLA

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

(57) Sargon Legend, 3 N-T 296, ll. 23-27 (Cooper and Heimpel 1983, 75, 77; Karahashi<br />

2000, 181)<br />

24. [i 7] ≠ma˙ i 7 mud±.ße 3 œa 2.ra For me, she drowned you in a great [river],<br />

mu.un.≠gir 5±.re.de 3.en a river of blood,<br />

25. [x] ≠x± [d] ≠ur±.[ d za].ba 4.ba 4<br />

… Urzababa chewed his lips, became<br />

nundum zu 2 bi 2.in.≠gu 7± seriously afraid,<br />

26. [x x] x na ≠AN± [x] sukkal.a.ni He spoke to … his chancellor,<br />

≠gu 3± mu.na.de 2.e<br />

27. […] ≠nin 9.e±.œu 10 ku 3 d inanna.ke 4<br />

“… my royal sister, Inanna,”<br />

(58) SP, UM 55-21-278 = 3 N-T 179 (Karahashi 2000, 181)<br />

ka 5.a zu 2 bi 2.in.sud 2.sud 2<br />

A fox bites.<br />

The extensive set of examples in this section show a regular case-marking pattern<br />

(adversely affected argument in the locative, *-a, while the positively affected argument<br />

occurs in the ergative, *-e) that differs from the case-marking pattern in the previous<br />

section in one detail: whereas the perceived object in BNBV inal verbs of perception<br />

regularly occurs in the locative-terminative case, *-e, the negatively affected argument in<br />

BNBV inal verbs of adversity regularly occur in the locative, *-a. I suspect that in the older<br />

phases of the language, the negatively affected argument in BNBV inal verbs of adversity<br />

was also followed by the locative-terminative, but that, unlike verbs of perception, the<br />

*bi-√ prefix in verbs of adversity caused the case-marking on the negatively affected<br />

73

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!