31.07.2015 Views

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Thinking Machines 127Taylor are grandmo<strong>the</strong>rs (indeed, a Jewish grandmo<strong>the</strong>r, in Taylor's case).When it comes to bachelors, many people—such as immigration authorities,justices of <strong>the</strong> peace, and health care bureaucrats—are notoriouslyMwfuzzy about who belongs in <strong>the</strong> category; as we all know, a lot canhinge on a piece of paper. Examples of unfuzzy thinking are everywhere.A judge may free an obviously guilty suspect on a technicality. Bartendersdeny beer to a responsible adult <strong>the</strong> day before his twenty-firstbirthday. We joke that you can't be a little bit pregnant or a little bit married,and after a Canadian survey reported that married women have sex1.57 times a week, <strong>the</strong> cartoonist Terry Mosher drew a woman sitting upin bed beside her dozing husband and muttering, "Well, that was .57."In fact, fuzzy and crisp versions of <strong>the</strong> same category can live side by sidein a single head. The psychologists Sharon Armstrong, Henry Gleitman,and Lila Gleitman mischievously gave <strong>the</strong> standard tests for fuzzy categoriesto university students but asked <strong>the</strong>m about knife-edged categorieslike "odd number" and "female." The subjects happily agreed to daft statementssuch as that 13 is a better example of an odd number than 23 is,and that a mo<strong>the</strong>r is a better example of a female than a comedienne is.Moments later <strong>the</strong> subjects also claimed that a number ei<strong>the</strong>r is odd or iseven, and that a person ei<strong>the</strong>r is female or is male, with no gray areas.People think in two modes. They can form fuzzy stereotypes by uninsightfullysoaking up correlations among properties, taking advantage of<strong>the</strong> fact that things in <strong>the</strong> world tend to fall into clusters (things that barkalso bite and lift <strong>the</strong>ir legs at hydrants). But people can also create systemsof rules—intuitive <strong>the</strong>ories—that define categories in terms of <strong>the</strong>rules that apply to <strong>the</strong>m, and that treat all <strong>the</strong> members of <strong>the</strong> categoryequally. All cultures have systems of formal kinship rules, often so precisethat one can prove <strong>the</strong>orems in <strong>the</strong>m. Our own kinship system givesus a crisp version of "grandmo<strong>the</strong>r": <strong>the</strong> mo<strong>the</strong>r of a parent, muffins bedamned. Law, arithmetic, folk science, and social conventions (with<strong>the</strong>ir rites of passage sharply delineating adults from children and husbandsfrom bachelors) are o<strong>the</strong>r rule systems in which people all over <strong>the</strong>planet reckon. The grammar of a language is yet ano<strong>the</strong>r.Rule systems allow us to rise above mere similarity and reach conclusionsbased on explanations. Hinton, Rumelhart, and McClelland wrote:"People are good at generalizing newly acquired knowledge. If, for example,you learn that chimpanzees like onions you will probably raise yourestimate of <strong>the</strong> probability that gorillas like onions. In a network thatuses distributed representations, this kind of generalization is auto-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!