31.07.2015 Views

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The <strong>Mind</strong>'s Eye | 237pen vertically in front of you, with <strong>the</strong> clip facing away at eleven o'clock.When you close each eye in turn, you will notice that only <strong>the</strong> left eyecan see <strong>the</strong> clip; it is hidden from <strong>the</strong> right eye by <strong>the</strong> rest of <strong>the</strong> pen.Was natural selection as astute as Leonardo when it designed <strong>the</strong> brain,letting it use this valuable clue to an object's boundary? Or does <strong>the</strong>brain ignore <strong>the</strong> clue, grudgingly chalking up each mismatch as anexception to <strong>the</strong> cohesive-matter assumption? The psychologists KenNakayama and Shinsuke Shimojo have shown that natural selection didnot ignore <strong>the</strong> clue. They created a random-dot stereogram whose depthinformation lay not in shifted dots but in dots that were visible in oneeye's view and absent in <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r's. Those dots lay at <strong>the</strong> corners of animaginary square, with dots at <strong>the</strong> top and bottom right comers only in<strong>the</strong> right eye's picture, and dots in <strong>the</strong> top and bottom left corners onlyin <strong>the</strong> left eye's picture. When people view <strong>the</strong> stereogram, <strong>the</strong>y see afloating square defined by <strong>the</strong> four points, showing that <strong>the</strong> brain indeedinterprets features visible to only one eye as coming from an edge inspace. Nakayama and <strong>the</strong> psychologist Barton Anderson suggest that<strong>the</strong>re arc neurons that detect <strong>the</strong>se occlusions; <strong>the</strong>y would respond to apair of marks in one eye, one of which can be matched with a mark in<strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r eye and <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r of which cannot be matched. These 3-Dboundary detectors would help a stereo network home in on <strong>the</strong> outlinesof <strong>the</strong> floating patches.Stereo vision does not come free with <strong>the</strong> two eyes; <strong>the</strong> circuitry has tobe wired into <strong>the</strong> brain. We know this because about two percent of <strong>the</strong>population can see perfectly well out of each eyeball but not with <strong>the</strong>cyclopean eye; random-dot stereograms remain flat. Ano<strong>the</strong>r four percentcan see stereo only poorly An even larger minority has more selectivedeficits. Some can't see stereo depth behind <strong>the</strong> point of fixation;o<strong>the</strong>rs can't see it in front. Whitman Richards, who discovered <strong>the</strong>seforms of stereoblindness, hypo<strong>the</strong>sized that <strong>the</strong> brain has three pools ofneurons that detect differences in <strong>the</strong> position of a spot in <strong>the</strong> two eyes.One pool is for pairs of spots that coincide exactly or almost exactly, forfine-grained depth perception at <strong>the</strong> point of focus. Ano<strong>the</strong>r is for pairsof spots Hanking <strong>the</strong> nose, for far<strong>the</strong>r objects. A third is for pairs of spotsapproaching <strong>the</strong> temples, for nearer objects. Neurons with all <strong>the</strong>se

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!