31.07.2015 Views

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

The <strong>Mind</strong>'s Eye | 289patients named only <strong>the</strong> buildings that would be visible on <strong>the</strong> right—neglecting <strong>the</strong> left half of imaginary space! Then <strong>the</strong> patients were askedto mentally walk across <strong>the</strong> square and stand on <strong>the</strong> ca<strong>the</strong>dral steps facing<strong>the</strong> piazza and describe what was in it. They mentioned <strong>the</strong> buildingsthat <strong>the</strong>y had left out <strong>the</strong> first time, and left out <strong>the</strong> buildings that <strong>the</strong>yhad mentioned. Each mental image depicted <strong>the</strong> scene from one vantagepoint, and <strong>the</strong> patients' lopsided window of attention examined <strong>the</strong>image exactly as it examined real visual inputs.These discoveries implicate <strong>the</strong> visual brain as <strong>the</strong> seat of imagery,and recently <strong>the</strong>re has been a positive identification. The psychologistStephen Kosslyn and his colleagues used Positron Emission Tomography(PET scanning) to see which parts of <strong>the</strong> brain are most active whenpeople have mental images. Each subject lay with his head in a ring ofdetectors, closed his eyes, and answered questions about uppercase lettersof <strong>the</strong> alphabet, such as whe<strong>the</strong>r B has any curves. The occipitallobe or visual cortex, <strong>the</strong> first gray matter that processes visual input, litup. The visual cortex is topographically mapped—it forms a picture, ifyou will. In some runs, <strong>the</strong> subjects visualized large letters, in o<strong>the</strong>rs,small letters. Pondering large letters activated <strong>the</strong> parts of <strong>the</strong> cortex representing<strong>the</strong> periphery of <strong>the</strong> visual field; pondering small letters activated<strong>the</strong> parts representing <strong>the</strong> fovea. Images really do seem to be laidacross <strong>the</strong> cortical surface.Could <strong>the</strong> activation be just a spillover of activity from o<strong>the</strong>r parts of <strong>the</strong>brain, where <strong>the</strong> real computation is being done? The psychologist MarthaFarah showed that it isn't. She tested a woman's ability to form mental imagesbefore and after surgery that removed her visual cortex in one hemisphere.After <strong>the</strong> surgery, her mental images shrank to half <strong>the</strong>ir normal width. Mentalimages live in <strong>the</strong> visual cortex; indeed, parts of images take up parts ofcortex, just as parts of scenes take up parts of pictures.Still, an image is not an instant replay. It lacks that pungency andtang, though not because it has been bleached or watered down: imaginingred is not like seeing pink. And curiously, in <strong>the</strong> PET studies <strong>the</strong>mental image sometimes caused more activation of <strong>the</strong> visual cortex thana real display, not less. Visual images, though <strong>the</strong>y share brain areas withperception, are somehow different, and perhaps that is not surprising.Donald Symons notes that reactivating a visual experience may well havebenefits, but it also has costs: <strong>the</strong> risk of confusing imagination with reality.Within moments of awakening from a dream, our memory for its plotis wiped out, presumably to avoid contaminating autobiographical mem-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!