31.07.2015 Views

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

70 HOW THE MIND WORKSstanding <strong>the</strong> popular misconception that we think in our mo<strong>the</strong>r tongue.As I showed in The Language Instinct, sentences in a spoken languagelike English or Japanese are designed for vocal communication betweenimpatient, intelligent social beings. They achieve brevity by leaving outany information that <strong>the</strong> listener can mentally fill in from <strong>the</strong> context. Incontrast, <strong>the</strong> "language of thought" in which knowledge is couched canleave nothing to <strong>the</strong> imagination, because it is <strong>the</strong> imagination. Ano<strong>the</strong>rproblem with using English as <strong>the</strong> medium of knowledge is that Englishsentences can be ambiguous. When <strong>the</strong> serial killer Ted Bundy wins astay of execution and <strong>the</strong> headline reads "Bundy Beats Date with Chair,"we do a double-take because our mind assigns two meanings to <strong>the</strong>string of words. If one string of words in English can correspond to twomeanings in <strong>the</strong> mind, meanings in <strong>the</strong> mind cannot be strings of wordsin English. Finally, sentences in a spoken language are cluttered witharticles, prepositions, gender suffixes, and o<strong>the</strong>r grammatical boilerplate.They are needed to help get information from one head to ano<strong>the</strong>r byway of <strong>the</strong> mouth and <strong>the</strong> ear, a slow channel, but <strong>the</strong>y are not neededinside a single head where information can be transmitted directly bythick bundles of neurons. So <strong>the</strong> statements in a knowledge system arenot sentences in English but ra<strong>the</strong>r inscriptions in a richer language ofthought, "mentalese."In our example, <strong>the</strong> portion of mentalese that captures family relationscomes in two kinds of statements. An example of <strong>the</strong> first is Alexfa<strong>the</strong>r-of Andrew: a name, followed by an immediate family relationship,followed by a name. An example of <strong>the</strong> second is Alex is-male: aname followed by its sex. Do not be misled by my use of English wordsand syntax in <strong>the</strong> mentalese inscriptions. This is a courtesy to you, <strong>the</strong>reader, to help you keep track of what <strong>the</strong> symbols stand for. As far as <strong>the</strong>machine is concerned, <strong>the</strong>y are simply different arrangements of marks.As long as we use each one consistently to stand for someone (so <strong>the</strong>symbol used for Alex is always used for Alex and never for anyone else),and arrange <strong>the</strong>m according to a consistent plan (so <strong>the</strong>y preserve informationabout who is <strong>the</strong> fa<strong>the</strong>r of whom), <strong>the</strong>y could be any marks in anyarrangement at all. You can think of <strong>the</strong> marks as bar codes recognized bya scanner, or keyholes that admit only one key, or shapes that fit only onetemplate. Of course, in a commercial computer <strong>the</strong>y would be patternsof charges in silicon, and in a brain <strong>the</strong>y would be firings in sets of neurons.The key point is that nothing in <strong>the</strong> machine understands <strong>the</strong>m <strong>the</strong>way you or I do; parts of <strong>the</strong> machine respond to <strong>the</strong>ir shapes; and are

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!