31.07.2015 Views

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Thinking Machines 85can be performed on <strong>the</strong> numbers with a few dronelike operations: lookingup entries in <strong>the</strong> addition table and carrying digits. Roman numeralshave not survived, except as labels or decorations, because addition operationsare far more complicated with <strong>the</strong>m, and multiplication and divisionoperations are practically impossible.Pinning down mental representations is <strong>the</strong> route to rigor in psychology.Many explanations of behavior have an airy-fairy feel to <strong>the</strong>mbecause <strong>the</strong>y explain psychological phenomena in terms of o<strong>the</strong>r, equallymysterious psychological phenomena. Why do people have more troublewith this task than with that one? Because <strong>the</strong> first one is "more difficult."Why do people generalize a fact about one object to ano<strong>the</strong>robject? Because <strong>the</strong> objects are "similar." Why do people notice thisevent but not that one? Because <strong>the</strong> first event is "more salient." Theseexplanations are scams. Difficulty, similarity, and salience are in <strong>the</strong>mind of <strong>the</strong> beholder, which is what we should be trying to explain. Acomputer finds it more difficult to remember <strong>the</strong> gist of Little Red RidingHood than to remember a twenty-digit number; you find it more difficultto remember <strong>the</strong> number than <strong>the</strong> gist. You find two crumpled balls ofl^ewspaper to be similar, even though <strong>the</strong>ir shapes are completely different,and find two people's faces to be different, though <strong>the</strong>ir shapes are- almost <strong>the</strong> same. Migrating birds that navigate at night by <strong>the</strong> stars in <strong>the</strong>sky find <strong>the</strong> positions of <strong>the</strong> constellations at different times of nightquite salient; to a typical person, <strong>the</strong>y are barely noticeable.But if we hop down to <strong>the</strong> level of representations, we find a firmersort of entity, which can be rigorously counted and matched. If a <strong>the</strong>oryof psychology is any good, it should predict that <strong>the</strong> representationsrequired by <strong>the</strong> "difficult" task contain more symbols (count em) or triggera longer chain of demons than those of <strong>the</strong> "easy" task. It should predictthat <strong>the</strong> representations of two "similar" things have more sharedsymbols and fewer nonshared symbols than <strong>the</strong> representations of "dissimilar"things. The "salient" entities should have different representationsfrom <strong>the</strong>ir neighbors; <strong>the</strong> "nonsalient" entities should have <strong>the</strong>same ones.Research in cognitive psychology has tried to triangulate on <strong>the</strong>mind's internal representations by measuring people's reports, reactiontimes, and errors as <strong>the</strong>y remember, solve problems, recognize objects,and generalize from experience. The way people generalize is perhaps<strong>the</strong> most telltale sign that <strong>the</strong> mind uses mental representations, and lotsof <strong>the</strong>m.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!