31.07.2015 Views

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

Steven Pinker -- How the Mind Works - Hampshire High Italian ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The <strong>Mind</strong>'s Eye \ 215benign illusion. Smears of ink or flickering phosphor dots can make uslaugh, cry, even become sexually aroused. Humans have made picturesfor at least thirty thousand years, and contrary to some social-sciencefolklore, <strong>the</strong> ability to see <strong>the</strong>m as depictions is universal. The psychologistPaul Ekman created a furor in anthropology by showing that isolatedNew Guinean highlanders could recognize <strong>the</strong> facial expressions in photographsof Berkeley students. (Emotions, like everything else, werethought to be culturally relative.) Lost in <strong>the</strong> brouhaha was a more basicdiscovery: that <strong>the</strong> New Guineans were seeing things in <strong>the</strong> photographsat all ra<strong>the</strong>r than treating <strong>the</strong>m as blotchy gray paper.The picture exploits projection, <strong>the</strong> optical law that makes perceptionsuch a hard problem. Vision begins when a photon (unit of light energy)is reflected off a surface and zips along a line through <strong>the</strong> pupil to stimulateone of <strong>the</strong> photoreceptors (rods and cones) lining <strong>the</strong> curved innersurface of <strong>the</strong> eyeball. The receptor passes a neural signal up to <strong>the</strong>brain, and <strong>the</strong> brain's first task is to figure out where in <strong>the</strong> world thatphoton came from. Unfortunately, <strong>the</strong> ray defining <strong>the</strong> photon's pa<strong>the</strong>xtends out to infinity, and all <strong>the</strong> brain knows is that <strong>the</strong> originatingpatch lies somewhere along <strong>the</strong> ray. For all <strong>the</strong> brain knows, it could be afoot away, a mile away, or many light-years away; information about <strong>the</strong>third dimension, distance from <strong>the</strong> eye, has been lost in <strong>the</strong> process ofprojection. The ambiguity is multiplied combinatorially by <strong>the</strong> milliono<strong>the</strong>r receptors in <strong>the</strong> retina, each fundamentally confused about howfar away its stimulating patch lies. Any retinal image, <strong>the</strong>n, could havebeen produced by an infinite number of arrangements of three-dimensionalsurfaces in <strong>the</strong> world (see <strong>the</strong> diagram on p. 9).Of course, we don't perceive infinite possibilities; we home in on one,generally close to <strong>the</strong> correct one. And here is an opening for a crafter ofillusions. Arrange some matter so that it projects <strong>the</strong> same retinal imageas an object <strong>the</strong> brain is biased to recognize, and <strong>the</strong> brain should haveno way of telling <strong>the</strong> difference. A simple example is <strong>the</strong> Victorian noveltyin which a peephole in a door revealed a sumptuously furnishedroom, but when <strong>the</strong> door was opened <strong>the</strong> room was empty. The sumptuousroom was in a dollhouse nailed to <strong>the</strong> door over <strong>the</strong> peephole.The painter-turned-psychologist Adelbert Ames, Jr., made a careerout of carpentering even stranger illusory rooms. In one, rods and slabswere suspended from wires higgledy-piggledy throughout <strong>the</strong> room. Butwhen <strong>the</strong> room was seen from outside through a peephole in a wall, <strong>the</strong>rods and slabs lined up into a projection of a kitchen chair. In ano<strong>the</strong>r

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!