12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

DEFINING VALIDITY 135<br />

involvement and in-depth responses of individuals<br />

secure a sufficient level of validity and reliability.<br />

This claim is contested by Hammersley (1992:<br />

144) and Silverman (1993: 153), who argue that<br />

these are insufficient grounds for validity and<br />

reliability, and that the individuals concerned<br />

have no privileged position on interpretation. (Of<br />

course, neither are actors ‘cultural dopes’ who<br />

need a sociologist or researcher to tell them<br />

what is ‘really’ happening!) Silverman (1993)<br />

argues that, while immediacy and authenticity<br />

make for interesting journalism, ethnography<br />

must have more rigorous notions of validity<br />

and reliability. This involves moving beyond<br />

selecting data simply to fit a preconceived or ideal<br />

conception of the phenomenon or because they<br />

are spectacularly interesting (Fielding and Fielding<br />

1986). Data selected must be representative of the<br />

sample, the whole data set, the field, i.e. they<br />

must address content, construct and concurrent<br />

validity.<br />

Hammersley (1992: 50–1) suggests that validity<br />

in qualitative research replaces certainty with<br />

confidence in our results, and that, as reality is independent<br />

of the claims made for it by researchers,<br />

our accounts will be only representations of that<br />

reality rather than reproductions of it.<br />

Maxwell (1992) argues for five kinds of validity<br />

in qualitative methods that explore his notion of<br />

‘understanding’:<br />

<br />

<br />

Descriptive validity (the factual accuracy of the<br />

account, that it is not made up, selective or<br />

distorted): in this respect validity subsumes<br />

reliability; it is akin to Blumenfeld-Jones’s<br />

(1995) notion of ‘truth’ in research – what<br />

actually happened (objectively factual).<br />

Interpretive validity (the ability of the research<br />

to catch the meaning, interpretations, terms,<br />

intentions that situations and events, i.e. data,<br />

have for the participants/subjects themselves,<br />

in their terms): it is akin to Blumenfeld-Jones’s<br />

(1995) notion of ‘fidelity’ – what it means to<br />

the researched person or group (subjectively<br />

meaningful); interpretive validity has no<br />

clear counterpart in experimental/positivist<br />

methodologies.<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

Theoretical validity (the theoretical constructions<br />

that the researcher brings to the research,<br />

including those of the researched): theory here<br />

is regarded as explanation. Theoretical validity<br />

is the extent to which the research explains<br />

phenomena; in this respect is it akin to construct<br />

validity (discussed below); in theoretical<br />

validity the constructs are those of all the<br />

participants.<br />

Generalizability (the view that the theory<br />

generated may be useful in understanding<br />

other similar situations): generalizing here<br />

refers to generalizing within specific groups<br />

or communities, situations or circumstances<br />

validly and, beyond, to specific outsider<br />

communities, situations or circumstances<br />

(external validity); internal validity has greater<br />

significance here than external validity.<br />

Evaluative validity (the application of an evaluative,<br />

judgemental of that which is being<br />

researched, rather than a descriptive, explanatory<br />

or interpretive framework). Clearly this<br />

resonates with critical-theoretical perspectives,<br />

in that the researcher’s own evaluative agenda<br />

might intrude.<br />

Both qualitative and quantitative methods can<br />

address internal and external validity.<br />

Internal validity<br />

Internal validity seeks to demonstrate that the<br />

explanation of a particular event, issue or set<br />

of data which a piece of research provides can<br />

actually be sustained by the data. In some degree<br />

this concerns accuracy, which can be applied to<br />

quantitative and qualitative research. The findings<br />

must describe accurately the phenomena being<br />

researched.<br />

In ethnographic research internal validity can<br />

be addressed in several ways (LeCompte and<br />

Preissle 1993: 338):<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

using low-inference descriptors<br />

using multiple researchers<br />

using participant researchers<br />

using peer examination of data<br />

Chapter 6

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!